Chapter 2
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & St. George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 7–29.
Bentz, C., Verkerk, A., Kiela, D., Hill, F., & Buttery, P. (2015). Adaptive communication: Languages with more non-native speakers tend to have fewer word forms. PLoS ONE 10(6): e0128254. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0128254.
Bentz, C., & Winter, B. (2013). Languages with more second language learners tend to lose nominal case. Language Dynamics and Change, 3, 1–27.
Brainard, M. S., & Doupe, A. J. (2002). What songbirds teach us about learning. Nature, 417, 351–358.
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 10, 439–448.
Brock, J. (2007.) Language abilities in Williams syndrome: a critical review. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 97–127.
Call, J. (2004). Inferences about the location of food in the great apes. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 232–241.
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.
Catchpole, C. L., & Slater, P. L. B. (1995). Bird song: Themes and variations. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origins and use. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 589–558.
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30, 352–366.
Corballis, M. (1999). The gestural origins of language. American Scientist, 87, 138–145.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.
De Waal, F. B. M., & Pollick, A. S. (2012). In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Everett, D. L. (2012). Language: The cultural tool. New York: Vintage.
Feher, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P., & Tchernichovski, O. (2009). De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature, 459, 564–568.
Fisher, S. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Monaco, A. P., & Pembrey, M. E. (1998). Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genetics, 18, 168–170.
Fisher, S. E., & Vernes, S. C. (2015). Genetics and the language sciences. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1, 289–310.
Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 258–267.
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Gibson, K. R. (2012). Language or protolanguage? A review of the ape literature. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). What language creation in the manual modality tells us about the foundations of language. The Linguistic Review, 22, 199–225.
Goldin-Meadow, S., McNeill, D., & Singleton, J. (1996). Silence is liberating: Removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality. Psychological Review, 103, 34–55.
Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 24, 8030–8035.
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M.B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50.
Gould, J. L., & Marler, P. (1987). Learning by instinct. Scientific American, 255, 74–85.
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68, 571–581.
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 439–444.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.
Hayes, C. (1951). The ape in our house. New York: Harper.
Hermann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills for social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.
Hock, H. H. (1986). Principles of historical linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203, 88–111
Hockett, C. F., & Altmann, S. (1968). A note on design features. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), Animal communication: Techniques of study and results of research. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment: A deficit in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 240–246.
Johnson, S. C., & Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folkbiology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 156–200.
Jürgens, U., Kirzinger, A., & von Cramon, D. Y. (1982). The effects of deep-reaching lesions in the cortical face area on phonation: A combined case report and experimental monkey study. Cortex, 18, 125–139.
Kegl, J., Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (1999). Creation through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In M. DeGraff (Ed.), Language creation and language change: Creolization, diachrony, and development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 10681–10686.
Knörnschild, M. (2014). Vocal production learning in bats. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 80–85.
Krause, J., Lalueza-Fox, C., Orlando, L., Enard, W., Green, R. E., Burbano, H. A., … Pääbo, S. (2007). The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology, 17, 1908–1912. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008
Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523.
Lieberman, P. H., Klatt, D. H., & Wilson, W. H. (1969). Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkeys and other nonhuman primates. Science, 164, 1185–1187.
Lipkind, D., Marcus, M. F., Bemis, D. K., Sasahara, K., Jacoby, N., Takahasi, M., … Tchernichovski, O. (2013). Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature, 498, 104–108.
Lizskowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173–187.
Lupyan, G., & Dale, R. (2016). Why are there different languages? The role of adaptation in linguistic diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 649–660.
Mervis, C. B. (1999). The Williams syndrome cognitive profile: Strengths, weaknesses, and interrelations among auditory short-term memory, language, and visuospatial constructive cognition. In E. Winograd, R. Fivush, & W. Hirst (Eds.), Ecological approaches to cognition: Essays in honour of Ulrich Neisser (pp. 193–228). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Messinger, D., Neal, R., & Schwartz, H. K. (2000) Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 283–298.
Mufwene, S. (2008). What do creoles and pidgins tell us about the evolution of language? In B. Laks, S. Cleziou, J. P. Demoule, & P. Encrevé (Eds.), The origins and evolution of languages: Approaches, models, paradigms (pp. 272–297). London: Equinox.
Okrent, A. (2010). In the land of invented language: Esperanto rock stars, Klingon poets, Loglan lovers and the mad dreamers who tried to build a perfect language. New York: Spiegel & Grau.
Owren, M. J., Amoss, R. T., & Rendall, D. (2011). Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 530–544.
Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R M., & Cheney, D. L. (1993). Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. Developmental Psychobiology, 26, 389–406.
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: HarperCollins.
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, 1–152.
Ralls, K., Fiorelli, P., & Gish, S. (1985). Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals: Phoca vitulina. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 63, 1050–1056.
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257.
Ridgway, S., Carder, D., Jeffries, M., & Todd, M. (2012). Spontaneous human speech mimicry by a cetacean. Current Biology, 22, R860–R861.
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C., & Aronoff, M. (2005). The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 2661–2665.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., & Boysen, S. (1980). Do apes use language? One research group considers the evidence for representational ability in apes. American Scientist, 68, 49–61.
Senghas, A. (2005). Language emergence: Clues from a new Bedouin sign language. Current Biology, 15, R463–R465.
Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (2001). Children creating language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science, 12, 323–328.
Senghas, A., Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2004). Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science, 305, 1779–1782.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2010). Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain and Language, 115, 92–100.
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 1070–1094.
Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication system of the American deaf (Studies in Linguistics: Occasional Papers No. 8). Buffalo, NY: Department of Anthropology & Linguistics, University of Buffalo.
Stooger, A. S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., de Silva, S., Herbst, C. T., Kwon, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2012). An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22, 2144–2148.
Su, S., Cai, F., Si, A., Zhang, S., Tautz, J., & Chen, S. (2008). East learns from West: Asiatic honeybees can understand dance language of European honeybees. PLoS ONE 3, e2365. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002365
Theisen, C. A.., Oberlander, J. & Kirby, S. (2009). Systematicity and arbitrariness in novel communication systems. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 31, 1971–1976.
Theisen, C. A., Oberlander, J., & Kirby, S. (2010). Systematicity and arbitrariness in novel communication systems. Interaction Studies, 11, 14–32.
Tomasello, M. (2006). Why don’t apes point? In N. Enfield & S. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality. New York: Wenner-Grenn.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Boston: MIT Press.
van der Lely, H. K., & Marshall, C. R. (2011). Grammatical-specific language impairment: A window onto domain-specificity. In J. Gouendouzi, F. Loncke, & M. J. Williams (Eds.), The handbook of psycholinguistics and cognitive processes: Perspectives in communication disorders (pp. 401–418). New York: Psychology Press.
Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Alcock, K., Fletcher, P., & Passingham, R. (1995). Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 930–933.
von Frisch, K. (1967). The dance language and orientation of bees. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Chapter 3
Ackerman, D. (2012). One hundred names for love: A memoir. New York: Norton.
Amunts, K., Lenzen, M., Friederici, A.D., Schleicher, A., Morosan, P., Palomero- Gallagher, N., & Zilles, K. (2010). Broca’s region: novel organizational principles and multiple receptor mapping. PLoS Biology, 8, e1000489.
Bates, E., Wilson, S., Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., Sereno, M., Knight, R. T., & Dronkers, N. F. (2003). Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 448–450.
Bentin, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 35–54.
Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M. H., Echallier, J. F., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 235–260.
Bierce, A. (1911). The devil’s dictionary. In The collected works of Ambrose Bierce (Vol. 7). New York: Neale.
Binder, J. R., Rao, S. M., Hammeke, T. A., Yetkin, M. D., Jesmanowicz, A., Bandettini, P. A., … Hyde, J. S. (1994). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human auditory cortex. Annals of Neurology, 35, 662–672.
Broca, P. P. (1861). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech). Bulletin de la Société Anatomique, 6, 330–357. Translation by Christopher D. Green, Retrieved from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Broca/aphemie-e.htm
Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergliechende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde (Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex). Translated and edited by L. J. Garey (1994). New York: Springer.
Brown, S., Martinez, M. J., & Parsons, L. M. (2006). Music and language side by side in the brain: A PET study of the generation of melodies and sentences. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 2791–2803.
Chao, L. L., Nielsen-Bohlman, L., & Knight, R. T. (1995). Auditory event-related potentials dissociate early and late memory processes. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 96, 157–168.
Chobert, J., Marie, C., Francois, C., Schon, D., & Besson, M. (2011). Enhanced passive and active processing of syllables in musician children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3874–3887.
De Bode, S., & Curtiss, S. (2000). Language after hemispherectomy. Brain and Cognition, 43, 145–148.
Desroches, A. S., Newman, R. L., & Joanisse, M. F. (2008). Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: Electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1893–1906.
Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., Utman, J. A., Dronkers, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2001). Language deficits, localization, and grammar: Evidence for a distributive model of language breakdown in aphasic patients and neurologically intact individuals. Psychological Review, 108, 759–788.
Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O. F., Iba-Zizen, M. T., & Cabanis, E. A. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic cases: High resolution MR images of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain, 130, 1432–1441.
Dronkers, N. F., Redfern, B. B., & Knight, R. T. (2000). The neural architecture of language disorders. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 949–958). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Jr., Redfern, B. B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92, 145–177.
Emmorey, K., McCollough, S., Mehta, S., Ponto, L. L. B., & Grabowski, T. J. (2011). Sign language and pantomime production differentially engage frontal and parietal cortices. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 878–901.
Fabbro, F. (2001). The bilingual brain: Bilingual aphasia. Brain and Language, 79, 201–210.
Gazzaniga, M. S., & Sperry, R. W. (1967). Language after section of the cerebral commissures. Brain, 90, 131–148.
Gierhan, S. M. E. (2013). Connections for auditory language in the human brain. Brain and Language, 127, 205–221.
Gouvea, A. C., Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Poeppel, D. (2010). The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 149–188.
Harlow, J. M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society, 2, 327–347.
Harrison, A. H., & Connolly, J. F. (2013). Finding a way in: A review and practical evaluation of fMRI and EEG for detection and assessment in disorders of consciousness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 1403–1419.
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.
Hickok, G. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 6, 121–143.
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 393–402.
Hickok, G., Bellugi, U., & Klima, E. S. (2001). Sign language in the brain. Scientific American, 284, 58–65.
Iacoboni, M., Freedman, J., Kaplan, J., Jamieson, K. H., Freedman, T., Knapp, B., & Fitzgerald, K. (2007, November 11). This is your brain on politics. New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2007/11/11/opinion/11freedman.html?pagewanted=all
Jones, J. L., Lucker, J., Zalewski, C., Brewer, C., & Drayna, D. (2009). Phonological processing in adults with deficits in musical pitch perception. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42, 226–234.
Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., von Cramon, D. Y., Zysset, S., Lohmann, G., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Bach speaks: A cortical “language network” serves the processing of music. NeuroImage, 17, 956–966.
Kos, M., Vosse, T., van den Brink, D., & Hagoort, P. (2010). About edible restaurants: Conflicts between syntax and semantics as revealed by ERPs. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, article 222. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00222/full
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205.
Larsen, B., Skinhøj, E., & Lassen, N. A. (1978). Variations in regional cortical blood flow in the right and left hemispheres during automatic speech. Brain, 101, 193–209.
Leny, R. (1793). Remarkable case of a boy, who lost a considerable portion of brain, and who recovered, without detriment to any faculty, mental or corporeal. Medical Commentaries, 8, 301–316.
Lopez, D. L. (2002). Snaring the fowler: Mark Twain debunks phrenology. Skeptical Inquirer, 26.1. Retrieved from http://www.csicop.org/si/show/snaring_the_fowler_mark_twain_debunks_phrenology.
Macmillan, M. (2008). Phineas Gage: Unravelling the myth. Psychologist, 21, 829–831.
MacSweeney, M., Capek, C. M., Campbell, R., & Woll, B. (2008). The signing brain: The neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 432–440.
Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Musical syntax is processed in Broca’s area: An MEG study. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 540–545.
Marner, L., Nyengaard, J. R., Tang, Y., & Pakkenberg, B. (2003). Marked loss of myelinated nerve fibers in the human brain with age. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 462, 144–152.
Meade, G., Midgley, K. J., Sevcikova Sehyr, Z., Holcomb, P. J., & Emmorey, K. (2017). Implicit co-activation of American Sign Language in deaf readers: An ERP study. Brain and Language, 170, 50–61.
Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Castro, S. L., & Besson, M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: More evidence for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 712–723.
Neville, H. J., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151–165.
Niedeggen, M., Roesler, F., & Jost, K. (1999). Processing of incongruous mental calculation problems: Evidence for an arithmetic N400 effect. Psychophysiology, 36, 302–324.
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806.
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., & Kraus, M. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear & Hearing, 30, 653–661.
Patel, A. D., Gibson, E., Ratner, J., Besson, M., & Holcomb, P. (1998). Processing syntactic relations in language and music: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 717–733.
Penfield, W., & Jasper, H. (1954). Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain. Boston: Little Brown & Co.
Piccirilli, M., Sciarma, T., & Luzzi, S. (2000). Modularity of music: Evidence from a case of pure amusia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 69, 541–545.
Poeppel, D. (2001). Pure word deafness and the bilateral processing of the speech code. Cognitive Science, 25, 679–693.
Raichle, M. E. (2000). A brief history of human functional brain mapping. In A. W. Toga & J. C. Mazziotta (Eds.), Brain mapping: The systems (pp. 33–77). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ross, E.D. (2010). Cerebral localization of functions and the neurology of language: Fact versus fiction or is it something else? Neuroscientist, 16, 222–243.
Ross, E. D., & Monnot, M. (2008). Neurology of affective prosody and its functional-anatomic organization in right hemisphere. Brain & Language, 104, 51–74.
Sadakata, M., & Sekiyama, K. (2011). Enhanced perception of various linguistic features by musicians: A cross-linguistic study. Acta Psychologica, 138, 1–10.
Sandrone, S., Bacigaluppi, M., Galloni, M. R., Cappa, S. F., Moro, A., Catani, M., … Martino, G. (2014). Weighing brain activity with the balance: Angelo Mosso’s original manuscripts come to light. Brain, 137, 621–633.
Schendan, H., Ganis, G., & Kutas, M. (1998). Neurophysiological evidence for visual perceptual organization of words and faces within 150 ms. Psychophysiology, 35, 240–251.
Schmithorst, V. J. (2005). Separate cortical networks involved in music perception: Preliminary functional MRI evidence for modularity of music processing. NeuroImage, 25, 444–451.
Sitnikova, T., Kuperberg, G., & Holcomb, P. J. (2003). Semantic integration in videos of real-world events: An electrophysiological investigation. Psychophysiology, 40, 160–164.
St. George, M., Kutas, M., Martinez, A., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Semantic integration in reading: Engagement of the right hemisphere during discourse processing. Brain, 122, 1317–1325.
Thompson, C. K. (2008). Treatment of syntactic and morphologic deficits in agrammatic aphasia: Treatment of underlying forms. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in adult aphasia (pp. 735–753). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Tremblay, P., & Dick, A. S. (2016). Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology. Brain and Language, 162, 60–71.
Turken, A. U., & Dronkers, N. (2011). The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001
Van Horn, J. D., Irimia, A., Torgerson, C. M., Chambers, M. C., Kikinis, R., & Toga, A. W. (2012). Mapping connectivity damage in the case of Phineas Gage. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e37454. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037454
Wang, Y., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A. (2001). Dichotic perception of Mandarin tones by Chinese and American listeners. Brain and Language, 78, 332–348.
Wernicke, C. (1968). The symptom complex of aphasia. Reprinted in English in Proceedings of the Boston Colloquium for the Philosophy of Science, 4, 34–97. (Original work published 1874)
West, P. (2008). The shadow factory. Santa Fe, NM: Lumen Books.
Chapter 4
Berent, I., Steriade, D., Lennertz, T., & Vaknin, V. (2007). What we know about what we have never heard: Evidence from perceptual illusions. Cognition, 104, 591–630.
Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Rathbun, K. (2005). Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16, 298–304.
Dupoux, E., Kakehi, K., Hirose, Y., Pallier, C., & Mehler, J. (1999). Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1568–1578.
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.
Emberson, L. L., Liu, R., & Zevin, J. D. (2013). Is statistical learning constrained by lower level perceptual organization? Congition, 128, 82–102.
Finn, A. S., & Hudson Kam, C. L. (2008). The curse of knowledge: First language knowledge impairs adult learners’ use of novel statistics for word learning. Cognition, 108, 477–499.
Fiser, J., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Unsupervised statistical learning of higher-order spatial structures from visual scenes. Psychological Science, 12, 499–504.
Hauser, M. D., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Segmentation of the speech stream in a non-human primate: Statistical learning in cotton-top tamarins. Cognition, 78, B53–B64.
Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1–23.
Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M. I., Svenkerud, V. Y. Z., & Jusczyk, A. M. (1993). Infants’ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 402–420.
Jusczyk, P. W., Hohne, E. A., & Bauman, A. (1999). Infant’s sensitivity to allophonic cues for word segmentation. Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 1465–1476.
Kudo, N., Nonaka, Y., Mizuno, N., Mizuno, K., & Okanoya, K. (2011). On-line statistical segmentation of a non-speech auditory stream as demonstrated by event-related potentials. Developmental Science, 14, 1100–1106.
Linzen, T., & Gallagher, G. (2017). Rapid generalization in phonotactic learning. Laboratory Phonology, 8, 1–32.
Mattys, S. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition, 78, 91–121.
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. A. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–11.
Mehler, J., Jussczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143–178.
Moreton, E., & Pater, J. (2012a). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning, Part I: Structure. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 686–701.
Moreton, E., & Pater, J. (2012b). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning, Part II: Substance. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 702–718.
Nelson, D. G. K., Jusczyk, P. W., Mandel, D. R., Myers, J., Turk, A., & Gerken, L. (1995). The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 111–116.
Peña, M., Pittaluga, E., & Mehler, J. (2010). Language acquisition in premature and full-term infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 3823–3828.
Peña, M., Werker, J. F., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2012). Earlier speech exposure does not accelerate speech acquisition. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 11159–11163.
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. M., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.
Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E., Aslin, R. N., & Newport,Th E. L. (1999). Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition, 70, 27–52.
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A., & Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8, 101–195.
Saffran, J. R., & Thiessen, E. D. (2003). Pattern induction by infant language learners. Developmental Psychology, 39, 484–494.
Sandler, W. (2017). The challenge of sign language phonology. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 43–63.
Shepard, R. N., Hovland, C. L., & Jenkins, H. M. (1961). Learning and memorization of classifications. Psychological Monographs 75 (13, Whole No. 517).
Teinonen, T., Fellman, V., Näätänen, R., Alku, P., & Huotilainen, M. (2009). Statistical language learning in neonates revealed by event-related brain potentials. BioMed Central Neuroscience, 10, 21. Retrieved from https://bmcneurosci.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2202-10-21.doi:10.1186/1471-2202-10-21
Toro, J. M., & Trobalón, J. B. (2005). Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 867–875.
Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75, 1067–1084.
Weikum, W. M., Oberlander, T. F., Hensch, T. K., & Werker, J. F. (2012). Prenatal exposure to antidepressants and depressed maternal mood alter trajectory of infant speech perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 17221–17227.
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization in the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.
White, K. S., Peperkamp, S., Kirk, C., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Rapid acquisition of phonological alternations by infants. Cognition, 107, 238–265.
Wilson, C. (2006). Learning phonology with substantive bias: An experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science, 30, 945–982.
Yoshida, K. A., Pons, F., Maye, J., & Werker, J. F. (2010). Distributional phonetic learning at 10 months of age. Infancy, 15, 420–433.
Chapter 5
Aksu-Koc, A., & Slobin, D. I. (1985). The acquisition of Turkish. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erblaum.
Annie E. Casey Foundation. (2013). Early warning! Why reading by the end of third grade matters. Annie E. Casey Foundation, Baltimore.: www.aecf.org/resources/early-warning-why-reading-by-the-end-of-third-grade-matters/
Arunachalam, S. (2013). Two-year-olds can begin to acquire verb meanings in socially impoverished contexts. Cognition, 129, 569-573.
Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395–418.
Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., Bill, B., Desjardins, R. N., Irwin, J. M., & Tidball, G. (1996). Infants’ reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. Child Development, 67, 3135–3153.
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57.
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177.
Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2009). A horse of a different color: Specifying with precision infants’ mappings of novel nouns and adjectives. Child Development, 80, 1, 15–22.
Callanan, M. A., Repp, A. M., McCarthy, M. G., & Latzke, M. A. (1994). Children’s hypotheses about word meanings: Is there a basic level constraint? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 108–138.
Cartmill, E. A., Armstrong, B. F., Gleitman, L. R., Goldin-Meadow, S., Medina, T. N., & Trueswell, J. C. (2013). Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 11278–11283.
Casasola, M. (2008). The development of infants’ spatial categories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 21–25.
Casasola, M., Bhagwat, J., & Burke, A. S. (2009). Learning to form a spatial category of tight-fit relations: How experience with a label can give a boost. Developmental Psychology, 45, 711–723.
Caselli, N. K., Sehyr, Z. S., Cohen-Goldberg, A. M., & Emmorey, K. (2017). ASL-LEX: A lexical database of American Sign Language. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 784–801.
Cazden, C. B. (1972). Child Language and Education. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 603–615.
Fennell, C. T., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What paradox? Referential cues allow for infant use of phonetic detail in word learning. Child Development, 81, 1376–1383.
Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. T. (2003). Early word-learners’ ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech, 46, 245–264.
Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: An advantage of words over tones. Child Development, 81, 472–479.
Fodor, J. A. (1998). Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. M. (1985). Implicit contrast in adjectives vs. nouns: Implications for word-learning in pre-schoolers. Journal of Child Language, 12, 125–143.
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73, 135–176.
Graf Estes, K. M., Evans, J., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R. (2007). Can infants map meanings to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science, 18, 254–260.
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6, 346–359.
Hallé, P. A., & de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1996). The format of representation of recognized words in infants’ early receptive lexicon. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 463–481.
Hammer, C. S., Lawrence, F. R., & Miccio, A. W. (2008). Exposure to English before and after entry into Head Start: Bilingual children’s receptive language growth in Spanish and English. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11, 30–56.
Hammer, C. S., Uchikoshi, Y., Gillanders, C., Castro, D., & Sandilos, L. E. (2014). The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 715–733.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., . . ., & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26, 1071–1083.
Hollich, G., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2007). Young children associate complex words with whole objects rather than salient parts. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1051–1061.
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365.
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2005). Imaging the past: Neural activation in the frontal and temporal regions during regular and irregular past-tense processing. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 282–296.
Justus, T., Larsen, J., Yang, J., de Mornay Davies, P., Dronkers, N., & Swick, D. (2011). The role of Broca’s area in regular past-tense morphology: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1–18.
Katz, N., Baker, E., & Macnamara, J. (1974). What’s in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. Child Development, 45, 469–473.
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009). The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 705–710.
Keller, H. (1909). The story of my life. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 483–524.
Koenig, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2010). 24-month-olds’ sensitivity to prior inaccuracy of the source: Possible mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 46, 815–826.
Kundera, M. (1980). The book of laughter and forgetting (M. H. Heim, Trans.). New York: Knopf.
Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.
Lentine, G., & Shuy, R. W. (1990). Mc-: Meaning in the marketplace. American Speech, 65, 349–366.
Lu, J., & Jones, A. (2016). The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native learners. Journal of Child Language, 43, 537–552.
MacNamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. Psychological Review, 79, 1–13.
Magid, R. W., & Pyers, J. E. (2017). “I use it when I see it”: The role of development and experience in Deaf and hearing children’s understanding of iconic gesture. Cognition, 162, 78–86.
Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York: Oxford University Press.
Maurer, D., Pathman, T., & Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science, 9, 316–322.
McDonough, L., Choi, S., & Mandler, J. M. (2003). Understanding spatial relations: Flexible infants, lexical adults. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 229–259.
Montag, J. L., Jones, M. N., & Smith, L. B. (2015). The words children hear: Picture books and the statistics for language learning. Psychological Science, 26, 1489–1496.
Naigles, L. R. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17, 357–364.
Orwell, G. (1946). Politics and the English language. Horizon, 13, 252–265.
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285–308.
Phillips, W., & Santos, L. R. (2007). Evidence for kind representations in the absence of language: Experiments with Rhesus monkeys (Macaca Mulatta). Cognition, 102, 445–463.
Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules. New York: HarperCollins.
Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought. New York: Penguin.
Quinn, P. C., & Johnson, M. H. (2000). Global-before-basic object categorization in connectionist networks and 2-month-old infants. Infancy, 1, 31–46.
Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D., & Rosenkrantz, S. L. (1993). Evidence for representations of perceptual similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants. Perception, 22, 463–475.
Rheingold, H. (2000). They have a word for it: A lighthearted lexicon of untranslatable words and phrases. Louisville, KY: Sarabande Books.
Ritzer, G. McUniversity in the postmodern consumer society. Quality in Higher Education, 2, 185–199.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tense of English verbs. In D. E. Rumelhart & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models (pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Schneidman, L. A., Arroyo, M. E., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). What counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language, 40, 672–686.
Soja, N. (1992). Inferences about the meanings of nouns: The relationship between perception and syntax. Cognitive Development, 7, 29–46.
Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381–382.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Kahana-Kalman, R., Baumwell, L., & Cyphers, L. (1998). Predicting variation in the timing of language milestones in the second year: An events history approach. Journal of Child Language, 25, 675–700.
Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., Woll, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). The road to language learning is iconic: Evidence from British Sign Language. Psychological Science, 23, 1443–1448.
Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2012). Six-month-olds comprehend words that refer to parts of the body. Infancy, 17, 432–444.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639–650.
Trueswell, J. C., Lin, Y., Armstrong, B., III, Cartmill, C. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Gleitman, L. R. (2016). Perceiving referential intent: Dynamics of reference in natural parent–child interactions. Cognition, 148, 117–135.
Ullman, M. T., Pancheva, R., Love, T., Yee, E., Swinney, D., & Hickok, G. (2005). Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: Evidence from the production, reading and judgment of inflection in aphasia. Brain and Language, 93, 185–238.
Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257–302.
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–2152.
White, K. S., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E., & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Adults show less sensitivity to phonetic detail in unfamiliar words, too. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 362–378.
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts. Cognition, 85, 223–250.
Xu, F., & Carey, S. (1996). Infants’ metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111–153.
Xu, F., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review, 114, 245–272.
Chapter 6
Ambridge, B., Rowland, C. F., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Comparing different accounts of inversion errors in children’s non-subject wh-questions: “What experimental data can tell us?” Journal of Child Language, 33, 519–557.
Bowerman, M. (1982). Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: Implications of developmental errors with causative verbs. Quaderni di Semantica, 3, 5–66.
Bowerman, M. (1988). The “no negative evidence” problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar? In J. A. Hawkins (Ed.), Explaining language universals. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Braine, M. D. S. (1971). On two types of models of the internalization of grammars. In D. J. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.
Brauer, J., Anwander, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Neuroanatomical prerequisites for language function in the maturing brain. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 459–466.
Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Developmental Psychology, 35, 29–44.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Crain, S., & Nakayama, M. (1987). Structure dependence in grammar formation. Language, 63, 522–543.
Culbertson, J., & Adger, D. (2014). Language learners privilege structured meaning over surface frequency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 5842–5847.
de Villiers, J. G., Roeper, T., & Vainikka, A. (1990). The acquisition of long distance rules. In L. Frazier & J. G. de Villiers (Eds.), Language processing and acquisition. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Diesel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 131–151.
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46, 621–646.
Fitz, H., & Change, F. (2017). Meaningful questions: The acquisition of auxiliary inversion in a connectionist model of sentence production. Cognition, 166, 225–250.
Franck, J., Rotondi, I., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2016). Learning structure-dependent agreement in a hierarchical artificial grammar. Journal of Memory and Language, 87, 84–104.
Friederici, A. D., Oberecker, R., & Brauer, J. (2012). Neurophysiological preconditions of syntax acquisition. Psychological Research, 76, 204–211.
Goldberg, A. E. (2015). Subtle implicit language facts emerge from the functions of constructions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2019. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02019
Gómez, R. L., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135.
Gómez, R. L., & Maye, J. (2005). The developmental trajectory of nonadjacent dependency learning. Infancy, 7, 183–206.
Lerdahl, F. (2005). Tonal pitch space. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Levin, B. (1993). English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lewis, J. D., & Elman, J. L. (2001). A connectionist investigation of linguistic arguments from the poverty of the stimulus: Learning the unlearnable. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 23. Retrieved from https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3wv86519
MacWhinney, B. (1975). Pragmatic patterns in child syntax. Stanford Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 10, 153–165.
Mintz, T. H. (2003). Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition, 90, 91–117.
Mintz, T. H. (2006). Finding the verbs: Distributional cues to categories available to young learners. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs. New York: Oxford University Press.
Novogrodsky, R., & Friedmann, N. (2006). The production of relative clauses in SLI: A window into the nature of the impairment. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 364–375.
Okrand, M. The Klingon dictionary: The official guide to Klingon words and phrases. New York: Pocket Books.
Okrent, A. (2009). In the Land of Invented Languages: Adventures in linguistic creativity, madness, and genius. New York: Spiegel & Grau.
Pearl, L., & Weinberg, A. (2007). Input filtering in syntactic acquisition: Answers from language change modeling. Language Learning and Development, 3, 43–72.
Perek, F., & Goldberg, A. E. (2015). Generalizing beyond the input: The functions of the constructions matter. Journal of Memory and Language, 84, 108–127.
Pinker, S. (1987). The bootstrapping problem in language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 399–441). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Redington, M., Chater, N., & Finch, S. (1998). Distributional information: A powerful cue for acquiring syntactic categories. Cognitive Science, 22, 435–469.
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V. M., & Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants of acquisition order in wh- questions: Re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech. Journal of Child Language, 30, 609–635.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tense of English verbs. In D. E. Rumelhart & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Vol 2. Psychological and biological models (pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Schuele, C. M., & Tolbert, L. (2001). Omissions of obligatory relative markers in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 15, 257–274.
Soderstrom, M., & Morgan, J. L. (2007). Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech. Developmental Science, 10, 641–653.
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A case study in early grammatical development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Tomasello, M., Akhtar, N., Dodson, K., & Rekau, L. (1997). Differential productivity in young children’s use of nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language, 24, 373–387.
Weisleder, A., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What’s in the input? Frequent frames in child-directed speech offer distributional cues to grammatical categories in Spanish and English. Journal of Child Language, 37, 1089–1108.
Wonnacott, E. (2011). Balancing generalization and lexical conservatism: An artificial language study with child learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 1–14.
Chapter 7
Adank, P., Hagoort, P., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Imitation improves language comprehension. Psychological Science, 21, 1903–1909.
Baese-Berk, M. M., Bradlow, A. R., & Wright, B. A. (2013). Accent-independent adaptation to foreign accented speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133, EL 174.
Baese-Berk, M., & Samuel, A. (2016). Listeners beware: Speech production may be bad for learning sounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 23–26.
Best, C. T., Matur, G., Miranda, K. A., & Lillo-Martin, M. (2010). Effects of sign language experience on categorical perception of dynamic ASL pseudosigns. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 747–762.
Boets, B., Vandermosten, M., Poelmans, H., Luts, H., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2011). Preschool impairments in auditory processing and speech perception uniquely predict future reading problems. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 560–570.
Bruderer, A. G., Danielson, D. K., Kandhadai, P., & Werker, J. F. (2015). Sensorimotor influences on speech perception in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 13531–13536.
Clopper, C. G., & Pisoni, D. B. (2004). Homebodies and army brats: Some effects of early linguistic experience and residential history on dialect categorization. Language Variation and Change, 16, 31–48.
Dekle, D., Fowler, C. A., & Funnell, M. (1992). Audio-visual integration in perception of real words. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 355–362.
Eisner, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2006). Perceptual learning in speech: Stability over time. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 1950–1953.
Eisner, F., Melinger, A., & Weber, A. (2013). Constraints on the transfer of perceptual learning in accented speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, article 148.
Fowler, C. A., & Dekle, D. (1991). Listening with eye and hand: Crossmodal contributions to speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 816–828.
Ganong, W. F. (1980). Phonetic categorization in auditory word perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 110–125.
Graf Estes, K., & Lew-Williams, C. (2015). Listening through voices: Infant statistical word segmentation across multiple speakers. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1517–1528.
Hasson, U., Skipper, J. I., Nusbaum, H. C., & Small, S. L. (2007). Abstract coding of audiovisual speech: Beyond sensory representation. Neuron, 56, 1116–1126.
Hockett, C. F. (1955). A manual of phonology (Indiana University Publications in Anthropology and Linguistics 11). Baltimore, MD: Waverly Press.
Holt, L. L., & Lotto, A. J. (2006). Cue weighting in auditory categorization: Implications for first and second-language acquisition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 3059–3071.
Houston, D. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1570–1582.
Kleinschmidt, D. F. (2018, under review). Structure in talker variability: How much is there and how much can it help? PsyArXiv. Retrieved from https://psyarxiv.com/a4tkn/.
Kraljic, T., & Samuel, A. G. (2007). Perceptual adjustments to multiple speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 1–15.
Kuhl, P. K., & Miller, J. D. (1975). Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction on alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190, 69–72.
Labov, W. (2010). Principles of linguistic change: Vol. 3. Cognitive and cultural factors. Chi Chester, England: Wiley-Blackwell.
Labov, W. (1973). The boundaries of words and their meanings. In C-J. N. Bailey & R. W. Shuy (Eds.), New ways of analyzing variation in English (pp. 340–371). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 358–368.
Lisker, L. (1986). “Voicing” in English: A catalogue of acoustic features signaling /b/ versus /p/ in trochees. Language and Speech, 29, 3–11.
Marie, C., Delogu, F., Lampis, G., Belardinelli, M. O., & Besson, M. (2011). Influence of musical expertise on segmental and tonal processing in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2401–2415.
Marie, C., Magne, C., & Besson, M. (2010). Musicians and the metric structure of words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2, 294–305.
Martínez-Montes, Hernández-Pérez, E., H., Chobert, J., Morgado-Rodríguez, L., Suárez-Murias, C., Váldez-Sosa, P. A., & Besson, M. (2013). Musical expertise and foreign speech perception. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7, article no 84. McMurray, B., Aslin, R. N., Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey, M. J., & Subik, D. (2008). Gradient sensitivity to within-category variation in words and syllables. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1609–1631.
Molfese, D. L. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72, 238–245.
Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24, 45–64.
Möttönen, R., & Watkins, K. E. (2009). Motor representations of articulators contribute to categorical perception of speech sounds. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9818–9825.
Myers, E. M., Blumstein, S. E., Walsh, E., & Eliassen, J. (2009). Inferior frontal regions underlie the perception of phonetic category invariance. Psychological Science, 20, 895–903.
Newman, R. S., Clouse, S. A., & Burnham, J. L. (2001). The perceptual consequences of within-talker variability in fricative production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 1181–1196.
Noordenbos, M. W., Segers, E., Serniclaes, W., Mitterer, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Allophonic mode of speech perception in Dutch children at risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 1469–1483.
Noordenbos, M. W., & Serniclaes, W. (2015). The categorical perception deficit in dyslexia: A meta-analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19, 340–359.
Nygaard, L. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Talker-specific learning in speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 355–376.
Panouilleres, M., & Mottonen, R. (2017). Decline of auditory-motor speech processing in older adults with hearing loss. bioRxiv, 169235. Retrieved from https://www.biorxiv.org/content/biorxiv/early/2017/08/31/169235.full.pdf
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., & Kraus N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and Hearing, 30, 653–661.
Peeters, M., Verhoeven, L., de Moor, J., & van Balkom, H. (2009). Importance of speech production for phonological awareness and word decoding: The case of children with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 712–726.
Perrachione, T. K., Del Tufo, S. N., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2011). Human voice recognition depends on language ability. Science, 333, 595–595.
Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2003). Cognitive aging and auditory information processing. International Journal of Audiology, 42, S26–S32.
Polka, L. (1992). Characterizing the influence of native language experience on adult speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 52, 37–52.
Potter, C. E., & Saffran, J. R. (2017). Exposure to multiple accents supports infants’ understanding of novel accents. Cognition, 166, 67–72.
Pulvermüller, F., Huss, M., Kherif, F., del Prado Martin, F. M., Hauk, O., & Shtyrov, Y. (2006). Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 7865–7870.
Reinisch, E., & Holt, L. L. (2014). Lexically guided phonetic retuning of foreign-accented speech and its generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 539–555.
Saltzman, D., & Myers, E. B. (2017). Listeners are maximally flexible in updating phonetic beliefs over time. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25, 718–724.
Schertz, J., Cho, T., Lotto, A., & Warner, N. (2015). Individual differences in phonetic cue use production and perception of a non-native sound contrast. Journal of Phonetics, 52, 183–204.
Schulz, A. A., Francis, A. L., & Llanos, F. (2012). Differential cue weighting in perception and production of consonant voicing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132, EL95–101.
Sekiyama, K., Soshi, T., & Sakamoto, S. (2014). Enhanced audiovisual integration with aging in speech perception: A heightened McGurk effect in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 323.
Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2004). Preference and processing: The role of speech affect in early spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 173–189.
Singh, L., White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Building a word-form lexicon in the face of variable input: Influences of pitch and amplitude on early spoken word recognition. Language Learning and Development, 4, 157–178.
Skipper, J. I., Devlin, J. T., & Lametti, D. R. (2015). The hearing ear is always found close to the speaking tongue: Review of the role of the motor system in speech perception. Brain and Language, 164, 77–105.
Slater, J., & Kraus, N. (2016). The role of rhythm in perceiving speech-in-noise: A comparison of percussionists, vocalists and non-musicians. Cognitive Processes, 17, 79–87.
Slater, J., Skoe, E., Strait, D. L., O’Connell, S., Thompson, E., & Kraus, N. (2015). Music training improves speech-in-noise training: Longitudinal evidence form a community-based music program. Behavioural Brain Research, 291, 244–252.
Snowling, M. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.
Stasenko, A., Bonn, C., Teghipco, A., Garcea, F. E., Sweet, C., Dombovy, M., … Mahon, B. Z. (2015). A causal test of the motor theory of speech perception: A case of impaired speech production and spared speech perception. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 32, 38–57.
Swaminathan, J., Mason, C. R., Streeter, T. M., Best, V., Kidd, G., Jr., & Patel, A. D. (2015). Musical training, individual differences and the cocktail party problem. Scientific Reports, 5, article no. 11628.
Tees, R. C., & Werker, J. F. (1984). Perceptual flexibility: Maintenance or recovery of the ability to discriminate non-native speech sounds. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 38, 579–590.
Tierney, A., Krizman, J., Skoe, E., Johnston, K., & Kraus, N. (2013). High school music classes enhance the neural processing of speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, article np. 855.
Toscano, J. C., & McMurray, B. (2010). Cue integration with categories: Weighting acoustic cues in speech using unsupervised learning and distributional statistics. Cognitive Science, 34, 434–464. van Heugten, M., & Johnson, E. K. (2014). Learning to contend with accents in infancy: Benefits of brief speaker exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 340–350.
Warren, R. M. (1970). Restoration of missing speech sounds, Science, 167, 392–393.
Xie, X., Theodore, R. M., & Myers, E. B. (2017). More than a boundary shift: Perceptual adaptation to foreign-accented speech reshapes the internal structure of phonetic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43, 206–217.
Zhao, T. C., & Kuhl, P. K. (2016). Musical intervention enhances infants’ neural processing of temporal structure in music and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 5212–5217.
Chapter 8
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.
Boulenger, V., Metchouff, L., Thobois, S., Broussolle, E., Jeannerod, M., & Nazir, T. A. (2008). Word processing in Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns. Neuropsychologia, 46, 743–756.
Carreiras, M., Gutierrez, E., Baquero, S., & Corina, D. P. (2008). Lexical processing in Spanish signed language (LSE). Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 100–122.
Caselli, N. K., & Cohen-Goldberg, A. M. (2014). Lexical access in sign language: A computational model. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 428. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00428
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual route and parallel distributed processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.
Davis, D. F., & Herr, P. M. (2014). Homophones as a phonological rote to priming. Journal of Consumer Research, 40, 1063–1077.
Duffy, S. A., Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K. (1988). Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 429–446.
Elman, J. L., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Cognitive penetration of the mechanisms of perception: Compensation for coarticulation of lexically restored phonemes. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 143–165.
Ferjan Ramirez, N., Leonard, M. K., Torres, C., Hatrak, M., Halgren, E., & Mayberry, R. I. (2014). Neural language processing in adolescent first-language learners. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 2772–2783.
Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ganong, W. F. (1980). Phonetic categorization in auditory word perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 110–125.
Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.
Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 501–518.
Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-acquisition effects in word and picture identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 684–712.
Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Claus, J. (2006). Beyond Vicary’s fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 792–798. Klatzky, R. L., Pellegrino, J. W., McCloskey, B. P., & Doherty, S. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments. Journal of Memory & Language, 28, 56–77.
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavioral Research, 44, 978–990. doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4
Laran, J., Dalton, A. N., & Andrade, E. B. (2011). The curious case of behavioral backlash: Why brands produce priming effects and slogans produce reverse priming effects. The Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 999–1014.
Luntz, F. I. (2007). Words that work: It’s not what you say, it’s what people hear. New York: Hyperion.
Magnuson, J. S., Dixon, J. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 133–156.
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97–115.
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 71–102.
Mayberry, R. I., Davenport, T., Roth, A., & Halgren, E. (2018). Neurolinguistic processing when the brain matures without language. Cortex, 99, 390–403.
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–86.
Meyer, D. A., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.
Morford, J. P., & Carlson, M. L. (2011). Sign perception and recognition in non-native signers of ASL. Language Learning and Development, 7, 149–168.
Myung, J., Blumstein, S. E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: Manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition, 98, 223–243.
Nelson, J. R., Liu, Y., Fiez, J., & Perfetti, C. A. (2009). Assimilation and accommodation patterns in ventral occipitotemporal cortex in learning a second writing system. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 810–820.
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2000). Merging information in speech recognition: Feedback is never necessary. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 299–325.
Perfetti, C., Nelson, J., Liu, Y., Fiez, J., & Tan, L. H. (2010). The neural basis of reading: Universals and writing system variations. In P. Cornelissen, P. Hansen, M. Kringelbach, & K. Pugh (Eds.), The neural basis of reading (pp. 147–172). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Phillips, C. I., Sears, C. R., & Pexman, P. M. (2012). An embodied semantic processing effect on eye gaze during sentence reading. Language and Cognition, 4, 99–114.
Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2012). The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition, 122, 280–291.
Pitt, M. A., & McQueen, J. M. (1998). Is compensation for coarticulation mediated by the lexicon? Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 347–370.
Pullum, G. K. (2012, January 15). Waterstones. Language Log. Retrieved from http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=3705
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: Twenty years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.
Slowiaczek, L. M., & Hamburger, M. (1992). Prelexical facilitation and lexical interference in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 1239 –1250.
Spivey, M., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10, 281–284.
Swinney, D. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645–659.
Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 8781–8785.
Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A rows is a rose: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory and Cognition, 15, 181–198.
Van Orden, G. C., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 14, 371–386.
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category-specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.
Weingarten, E., Chen, Q., McAdams, M., Hepler, J., & Yi, J. (2016). From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 472–497.
Yee, E., & Sedivy, J. (2006). Eye movements to pictures reveal transient semantic activation during spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 32, 1–14.
Zwitserlood, P. (1989). The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing. Cognition, 32, 25–64.
Chapter 9
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. J., Chaudhuri, J. R, & Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigrant status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944.
Allbritton, D. W., McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1996). Reliability of prosodic cues for resolving syntactic ambiguity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 714–735.
Altmann, G. T. A., & Steedman, M. (1988). Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition, 30, 191–238.
Bekinschtein, T. A., David, M. H., Rodd, J. M., & Owen, A. M. (2011). Why clowns taste funny: The relationship between humor and semantic ambiguity. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9665–9671.
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures, in J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley, 279–362.
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 233–262.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303.
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 417–436.
Brown, J. C. (1960). Loglan. Scientific American, 202, 43–63.
Britt, M. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1992). Parsing in discourse: Context effects and their limits. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 293–314..
Chambers, C. G., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S. (2004). Actions and affordances in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 687–696.
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 181–204.
Crain, S., & Steedman, M. (1985). On not being led up the garden path: The use of context by the psychological parser. In D. Dowty, L. Karttunen, & A. Zwicky (Eds.), Natural language parsing: Psychological, computational and theoretical perspectives (pp. 320–358). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–456.
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1117–1121.
Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44, 491–505.
Frank, S. L., Otten, L. J., Galli, G., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). The ERP response to the amount of information conveyed by words in sentences. Brain and Language, 140, 1–11.
Frazier, L., & Clifton, C., Jr. (1996). Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage model of parsing. Cognition, 6, 291–325.
Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (1987). Resolution of syntactic category ambiguities: Eye movements in parsing lexically ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 505–526.
Gibson, E. (1998). Syntactic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76.
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Recall of rapidly presented random chess positions is a function of skill. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 159–163.
Gopnik, A. (2011). Winter: Five windows on the season (CBC Massey Lecture). Toronto: House of Anansi Press.
Hale, J. (2001). A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic model. Proceedings of the Second Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 2, 159–166.
Henderson, J. M., Choi, W., Lowder, M. W., & Ferreira, F. (2016). Language structure in the brain: A fixation-related fMRI study of syntactic surprisal in reading. NeuroImage, 132, 293–300.
Hofweber, J., Marinis, T., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2016). Effects of dense code-switching on executive control. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6, 648–668.
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2010). Cascading activation across levels of representation in children’s lexical processing. Journal of Child Language, 38, 644–661.
Huettig, F., & Mani, N. (2016). Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 19–31.
Huettig, F., Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2011). Language-mediated visual orienting behavior in low and high literates. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 285.
Hussey, E. K., Harbison, J. I., Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Michler, A., Velnoskey, K., & Novick, J. M. (2017). Memory and language improvements following cognitive control training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 43, 23–58.
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in highly-proficient bilinguals? Acta Psychologica, 127, 277–288.
Janus, M., Lee, Y., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Effects of short-term music and second-language training on executive control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 84–97.
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133–156.
Kovács, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 6556–6560.
Kraljic, T., & Brennan, S. E. (2005). Prosodic disambiguation of syntactic structure: For the speaker or for the addressee? Cognitive Psychology, 50, 194–231.
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., & de Bruin, A. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 394–425.
Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 1126–1177.
Levy, R., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2013). The syntactic complexity of Russian relative clauses. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 461–495.
Lewis, R. L. (1996). Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 93–115.
MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: A comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 35–54.
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676–703.
Mahowald, K. & Fedorenko, E. (2016). Reliable individual-level neural markers of high-level language processing: A necessary precursor for relating neural variability to behavioral and genetic variability. NeuroImage, 139, 74–93.
Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J. R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 574–588.
Miller, L. M. S. (2009). Age differences in the effects of domain knowledge on reading efficiency. Psychology and Aging, 24, 63–74.
Mitchell, D. C., & Cuetos, F. (1991). The origins of parsing strategies. In C. Smith (Ed.), Current issues in natural language processing (pp. 1–12). Austin: University of Texas, Center for Cognitive Science.
Mitsugi, S., & MacWhinney, B. (2016). The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 19–35.
Montag, J. L., & MacDonald, M. C. (2015). Text exposure predicts spoken production of complex sentences in 8- and 12-year-old children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 447–468.
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719–726.
Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: Re-examining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 53, 263–281.
Okrent, A. (2009). In the land of invented languages. New York: Random House.
Payne, B. R., Gao, X., Noh, S. R., Anderson, C. J., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2012). The effects of print exposure on sentence processing and memory in older adults: Evidence for efficiency and reserve. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 122–149.
Payne, B. R., Grison, S., Gao, X., Christianson, K., Morrow, D. G., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2014). Aging and individual differences in binding during sentence understanding: Evidence from temporary and global syntactic attachment ambiguities. Cognition, 130, 157–173.
Rogers, C. L., Lister, J. J., Febo, D. M., Besing, J. M., & Abrams, H. B. (2006). Effects of bilingualism, noise, and reverberation on speech perception by listeners with normal hearing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 465–485.
Roland, D., Dick, F., & Elman J. L. (2007). Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 348–379.
Schafer, A. J., Speer, S., Warren, P., & White, S. D. (2000). Intonational disambiguation in sentence production and comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 169–182.
Shafto, M. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2014). Language in the aging brain: The network dynamics of cognitive decline and preservation. Science, 346, 583–587.
Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17, 103–186.
Smith, Z. (2009). Brief interviews with hideous men: The difficult gifts of David Foster Wallace. In Changing my mind: Occasional essays (pp. 255–297). New York: Penguin Press.
Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Trueswell, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1993). Context effects in syntactic ambiguity resolution: Parsing reduced relative clauses. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 47, 276–309.
Sprouse, J., Wagers, M., & Philips, C. (2012). A test of the relation between working memory capacity and syntactic island effects. Language, 87, 274–288.
Street, J. A., & Da˛browska, E. (2010). More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua, 20, 2080–2094.
Tanenhaus, M. K., Boland, J., E., Garnsey, S. M., & Carlson, G. N. (1989). Lexical structure in parsing long-distance dependencies. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 37–50.
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634.
Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Mishler, A., Corbett, R., Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Trueswell, J. C., & Novick, J. M. (2016). The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery. Cognition, 150, 213–231.
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Ramscar, M., & Chrysikou, E. G. (2009). Cognition without control: When a little frontal lobe goes a long way. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 259–263.
Trueswell, J. C. (1996). The role of lexical frequency in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 566–585.
Trueswell, J. C., & Kim, A. E. (1998). How to prune a garden path by nipping it in the bud: Fast priming of verb argument structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 102–123.
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73, 89–134.
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 285–318.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.
Van Dyke, J. A., Johns, C. L., & Kukona, A. (2014). Low working memory capacity is only spuriously related to poor reading comprehension. Cognition, 131, 373–403.
Woodard, K., Pozzan, L., & Trueswell, J. C. (2016). Taking your own path: Individual differences in executive function and language processing skills in child learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 187–209.
Chapter 10
Arnold, J. E., Fagnano, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2003). Disfluencies signal theee, um, new information. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 25–36.
Arnold, J. E., Hudson Kam, C. L., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007). If you say Thee uh you are describing something hard: The online attribution of disfluency during reference comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 914–930.
Arnold, J. E., Wasow, T., Asudeh, A., & Alrenga, P. (2004). Avoiding attachment ambiguities: The role of constituent ordering. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 55–70.
Baars, B. J., Motley, M. J., & MacKay, D. G. (1975). Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 382–391.
Bock, J. K. (1986a). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387.
Bock, J. K. (1986b). Meaning, sound, and syntax: Lexical priming in sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 575–586.
Brennan, S. E. (1990). Conversation as direct manipulation: An iconoclastic view. In B. K. Laurel (Ed.), The art of human–computer interface design (pp. 393–404). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Brown, R., & McNeill, D. (1966). The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 325–337.
Brown-Schmidt, S., & Konopka, A. E. (2008). Little houses and casas pequeñas: Message formulation and syntactic form in unscripted speech with speakers of English and Spanish. Cognition, 109, 274–280.
Brown-Schmidt, S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2006). Watching the eyes when talking about size: An investigation of message formulation and utterance planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 592–609.
Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. It’s the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition, 105, 658–668.
D’Angelo, M. A., & Humphreys, K. R. (2015). Tip-of-the-tongue states reoccur because of implicit learning, but resolving them helps. Cognition, 142, 166–190.
Dall, R., Tomalin, M., Wester, M., Byrne, W., & King, S. (2014, September). Investigating automatic and human filled pause insertion for speech synthesis. Presented at the Fifteenth Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association, Singapore.
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321.
Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1981). Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 611–629.
Erard, M. (2008). Um… . Slips, stumbles, and verbal blunders, and what they mean. New York: Anchor Books.
Ferreira, V. S. (1996). Is it better to give than to donate? Syntactic flexibility in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 724–755.
Fox Tree, J. E. (2001). Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 29, 320–326.
Fox Tree, J. E. (2002). Interpreting pauses and ums at turn exchanges. Discourse Processes, 34, 37–55.
Fraundorf, S. H., & Watson, D. G. (2011). The disfluent discourse: Effects of filled pauses on recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 161–175.
Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47, 27–52.
Gardner, R. (2010). Question and answer sequences in Garrwa talk. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 30, 423–445.
Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production (Vol. 1, pp. 177–220. London: Academic Press.
Gleitman, L., January, D., Nappa, R., & Trueswell, J. C. (2007). On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 544–569.
Gries, S. T. (2005). Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34, 365–399.
Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274–279.
Harley, T. A. (1984). A critique of top-down independent level models of speech production: Evidence from non-plan-internal speech errors. Cognitive Science, 8, 191–219.
Harley, T. A., & Bown, H. E. (1998). What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighbourhood effects in speech production. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 151–174.
Hawkins, P. R. (1971). The syntactic location of hesitation pauses. Language and Speech, 14, 277–288.
Humphreys, K. R., Menzies, H., & Lake, J. K. (2010). Repeated speech errors: Evidence for learning. Cognition, 117, 151–165.
Hwang, H., & Kaiser, E. (2009). The effects of lexical vs. perceptual primes on sentence production in Korean: An on-line investigation of event apprehension and sentence formulation. Paper presented at the 22nd CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Davis, CA.
Jaeger, T. F., Furth, K., & Hilliard, C. (2012). Phonological overlap affects lexical selection during sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1439–1449.
Jaeger, T. F., & Norcliffe, E. J. (2009). The cross-linguistic study of sentence production. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3/4, 866–887.
Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Priming sentence planning. Cognitive Psychology, 73, 1–40.
Lehtonen, J., & Sajavaara, K. (1985). The silent Finn. In D. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike (Eds.), Perspectives on silence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Martin, N., Weisberg, R. W., & Saffran, E. M. (1989). Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: Implications for models of lexical retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 462–485.
McMenamin, G. R. (2010). Theory and practice of forensic stylistics. In M. Coulthard & A. Johnson (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of forensic linguistics. New York: Routledge.
Meyer, A. S. (1996). Lexical access in phrase and sentence production: Results from picture–word interference experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 477–496.
Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). The selection of lexical-syntactic features in noun phrase production: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 907–922.
Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1976). Laboratory induction of verbal slips: A new method for psycholinguistic research. Communication Quarterly, 24, 28–34.
Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1979). Effects of cognitive set upon laboratory induced verbal (Freudian) slips. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 21–432.
Motley, M. T., Camden, C. T., & Baars, B. J. (1982). Covert formulation and editing of anomalies in speech production: Evidence from experimentally elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 503–522.
Myachykov, A., Garrod, S., & Scheepers, C. (2010). Perceptual priming of structural choice during English and Finnish sentence production. In R. K. Mishra & N. Srinivasan (Eds.), Language & Cognition: State of the Art (pp. 54–72). Munich: Lincom Europa.
Norcliffe, E., Konopka, A. E., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Word order affects the time course of sentence formulation in Tzeltal. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 1187–1208.
Roberts, F., Margutti, P., & Takano, S. (2011). Judgments concerning the valence of inter-turn silence across speakers of American English, Italian, and Japanese. Discourse Processes, 48, 331–354.
Smith, M., & Wheeldon, L. (1999). High-level processing scope in spoken sentence production. Cognition, 73, 205–246.
Stallings, L. M., & MacDonald, M. C. (2011). It’s not just the “heavy NP”: Relative phrase length modulates the production of heavy-NP shift. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40, 177–187.
Stivers, T., Enfielf, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., … Levinson, S. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 10587–10592.
Vigliocco, G., Antonini, T., & Garrett, M. F. (1997). Grammatical gender is on the tip of Italian tongues. Psychological Science, 8, 314–318.
Vitevitch, M. S. (2002). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28, 735–747.
Wagner, V., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H. (2010). On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: Effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 36, 423–440.
Walsh, M. (1997). Cross cultural communication problems in Aboriginal Australia. Discussion Paper 7/1997. Darwin: North Australia Research Unit.
Warriner, A. B., & Humphreys, K. R. (2008). Learning to fail: Reoccurring tip-of-the-tongue states. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 535–542.
Watanabe, M., Hirose, K., Den, Y., & Minematsu, N. (2008). Filled pauses as cues to the complexity of upcoming phrases for native and non-native listeners. Speech Communication, 50, 81–94.
Womack, K., McCoy, W., Alm, C. O., Calvelli, C., Pelz, J. B., Shi, P., & Haake, A. (2012). Disfluencies as extra-propositional indicators of cognitive processing. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Extra-Propositional Aspects of Meaning in Computational Linguistics (pp. 1–9). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics.
Chapter 11
Almor, A. (1999). Noun-phrase anaphora and focus: The informational load hypothesis. Psychological Review, 106, 748–765.
Almor, A., & Eimas, P. D. (2008). Focus and noun phrase anaphors in spoken language comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 201–225.
Arnold, J. E. (2001). The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes, 31, 137–162.
Arnold, J. E., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. (2007). Children’s use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 527–565.
Arnold, J. E., Eisenband, J., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eye tracking. Cognition, 76, B13–B26.
Arnold, J. E., Strangmann, I. M., Hwang, H., Zerkle, S., & Nappa, R. (2018). Linguistic experience affects pronoun interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 102, 41–54.
Badecker, W., & Straub, K. (2002). The processing role of structural constraints on the interpretation of pronouns and anaphors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 748–769.
Black, J. B., & Bern, H. (1981). Causal coherence and memory for events in narratives. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 267–275.
Blasko, D. G., & Connine, C. M. (1993). Effects of familiarity and aptness on metaphor processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 295–308.
Blatchford, C. (2011, November 28). “This is Canada. Do not be afraid,” relative told alleged honour killing victim. National Post.
Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P. (2003). Psychonarratology: Foundations for the Empirical Study of Literary Response. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R., & Franks, J. J. (1972). Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 193–209.
Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigation of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.
Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Mahoney, C. R., Walters, E. K., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). You heard it here first: Readers mentally simulate described sounds. Acta Psychologica, 135, 209–215.
Chiappe, D. L., & Chiappe, P. (2007). The role of working memory in metaphor production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 172–188.
Chien, Y. C., & Wexler, K. (1990). Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition, 1, 225–295.
Clackson, K., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2011). Children’s processing of reflexives and pronouns in English: Evidence from eye movements during listening. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 128–144.
Cui, X., Jeter, C. B., Yang, D., Montague, P. R., & Eagleman, D. M. (2007). Vividness of mental imagery: Individual variation can be measured objectively. Visual Research, 47, 474–478.
de Bonis, M., & Epelbaum, C. (1997). The comprehension of metaphors in schizophrenia. Psychopathology, 30, 149–154.
Denis, M. (1982). Imaging while reading text: A study of individual differences. Memory and Cognition, 10, 540–545.
Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good-enough representations in language processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 11–15.
Fiedler, K., & Walthier, E. (1996). Do you really know what you have seen? Intrusion errors and presuppositions effects on constructive memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 484–511.
Fitzgerald, F. S. (1922). The curious case of Benjamin Button. In Tales of the Jazz Age. New York: Scribner.
Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. (1977). Interpreting anaphoric relations: The integration of semantic information while reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Memory, 16, 77–90.
Garvey, C., & Caramazza, A. (1974). Implicit causality in verbs. Linguistic Inquiry, 4–5, 459–464.
Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155–170.
Gentner, D. (1988). Metaphor as structure mapping: The relational shift. Child Development, 59, 47–59.
Gernsbacher, M., Keysar, B., Robertson, R., & Werner, N. (2001). The role of suppression and enhancement in understanding metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 433–450.
Gerrig, R. J., Love, J., & McKoon, G. (2009). Waiting for Brandon: How readers respond to small mysteries. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 144–153.
Giannetti, L., & Eyman, S. (1986). Flashback: A brief history of film. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83.
Glucksberg, S., & Keysar, B. (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review, 97, 3–18.
Glucksberg, S., Gildea, P., & Bookin, H. B. (1982). On understanding nonliteral speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 85–98.
Gordon, P. C., Grosz, B. J., & Gilliom, L. A. (1993). Pronouns, names, and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311–347.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3. Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.
Guillaume, P. (1963). Manuel de psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Happé, F. G. E. (1991). The autobiographical writings of three Asperger syndrome adults: Problems of interpretation and implications for theory. In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 207–242). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Haviland, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1974). What’s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 512–521.
Hemon, A. (2012, September 10). Beyond the matrix. The New Yorker.
Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children’s narratives: A comparison of English, French, German and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 26, 419–452.
Hirsch, E. D., Jr. (2003, Spring). Reading comprehension requires knowledge of words and the world: Scientific insights into the fourth-grade slump and the nation’s stagnant comprehension scores. American Educator 10, 44.
Hoffman, R. R., & Kemper, S. (1987). What could reaction-time studies be telling us about metaphor comprehension? Metaphor and Symbol, 2, 149–186.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic.
Johnston, P. (1984). Prior knowledge and reading comprehension test bias. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 219–239.
Kehler, A., & Rohde, H. (2013). A probabilistic reconciliation of coherence-driven and centering-driven theories of pronoun interpretation. Theoretical Linguistics, 39(1–2), 1–37.
Kim, S. (1999). Causal bridging inference: A cause of story interestingness. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 57–71.
Kintsch, W. (2000). Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 257–266.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
LaMattina, J. (2012). The Artist: Michel Hazanavicius. http://vimeo.com/37187304
Linderholm, T. (2002). Predictive inference generation as a function of working memory capacity and causal text constraints. Discourse Processes, 34, 259–280.
Loftus, E., Miller, D., & Burns, H. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31.
Love, J., & McKoon, G. (2011). Rules of engagement: Complete and incomplete pronoun resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 874–887.
MacDonald, M. C., & Just, M. A. (1989). Changes in activation levels with negation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 633–644.
Marschark, M., & Cornoldi, C. (1991). Imagery and verbal memory. In C. Cornoldi & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Imagery and cognition (pp. 133–182). New York: Springer.
McElree, B., & Nordlie, J. (1999). Literal and figurative interpretations are computed in equal time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 486–494.
McEwan, I. (1978). In between the sheets. London: Cape.
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1986). Inferences about predictable events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 82–91.
McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 22, 247–288.
McNamara, D. S., & O’Reilly, T. (2009). Theories of comprehension skill: Knowledge and strategies versus capacity and suppression. In A. M. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (p. 62). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Megherbi, H., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2009). The online interpretation of pronouns and repeated names in seven-year-old children. Current Psychology Letters, 25, 2–11.
Morrison, T. (1987). Beloved. New York: Knopf.
Myers, J. L., Shinjo, M., & Duffy, S. A. (1987). Degree of causal relatedness and memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 453–465.
O’Brien, E. J., Rizella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. E. (1998). Updating a situation model: A memory-based processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1200–1210.
Oakhill, J., & Yuill, N. (1986). Pronoun resolution in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders: Effects of memory load and inferential complexity. Language and Speech, 29, 25–37.
Rapp, D. N., & Gerrig, R. J. (2006). Predilections for narrative outcomes: The impact of story contexts and reader preferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 54–67.
Rymer, R. (2012, July). Vanishing voices. National Geographic.
Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children’s interpretation of pronouns. Lingua, 117, 1959–1987.
Speer, N. K., Reynolds, J. R., Swallow, K. M., & Zacks, J. M. (2009). Reading stories activates neural representations of visual and motor experiences. Psychological Science, 20, 989–999.
St. George, M., Mannes, S., & Hoffman, J. E. (1994). Global semantic expectancy and language comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 70–83.
Stanton, A. (2012). The clues to a great story. (Talk presented at the TED conference, Long Beach California.) http://www.ted.com/talks/andrew_stanton_the_clues_to_a_great_story.html
Steen, G. J., Reijnierse, W. G., & Burgers, C. (2014). When do natural language metaphors influence reasoning? A follow-up study to Thibodeau and Boroditsky (2013). PLoS ONE, 9, e113536. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0113536
Stevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 519–548.
Thibodeau, P. H., & Boroditsky, L. (2011). Metaphors we think with: The role of metaphor in reasoning. PLoS ONE, 6, e16782. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016782
Thibodeau, P. H., & Boroditsky, L. (2013). Natural language metaphors covertly influence reasoning. PLoS ONE, 8, e52961. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052961
Trabasso, T., & Suh, S. (1993). Understanding text: Achieving explanatory coherence through on-line inferences and mental operations in working memory. Discourse Processes, 16, 3–34.
Van Berkum, J. J. A., Zwitserlood, P., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (2003). When and how do listeners relate a sentence to the wider discourse? Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 701–718.
Zwaan, R. A. (1996). Processing narrative time shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1196–1207.
Chapter 12
Abbot-Smith, K., Nurmsoo, E., Croll, R., Ferguson, H., & Forrester, M. (2016). How children aged 2;6 tailor verbal expressions to interlocutor informational needs. Journal of Child Language, 43, 1277–1291.
Arnold, J. E., Bennetto, L., & Diehl, J. J. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition, 110, 131–146.
Arnold, J. E., Wasow, T., Asudeh, A., & Alrenga, P. (2004). Avoiding attachment ambiguities: The role of constituent ordering. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 55–70.
Bardi, L., Six, P., & Brass, M. (2017). Repetitive TMS of the temporo-parietal junction disrupts participant’s expectations in a spontaneous theory of mind task. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1775–1782.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.
Barr, D. J. (2008). Pragmatic expectations at linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground. Cognition, 109, 18–40.
Barr, D. J., & Keysar, B. (2002). Anchoring comprehension in linguistic precedents. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 391–418.
Bašnáková, J., Weber, K., Petersson, K. M., van Berkum, J., & Hagoort, P. (2014). Beyond the language given: Neural correlates of inferring speaker meaning. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 2572–2578.
Bell, A., Brenier, J. M., Gregory, M., Girand, C., & Jurafsky, D. (2009). Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 92–111.
Bloom, P. (2002) Mindreading, communication, and the learning of names for things. Mind and Language, 17, 37–54.
Bott, L., & Noveck, I. A. (2004). Some utterances are underinformative: The onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 437–457.
Breheny, R., Ferguson, H. J., & Katsos, N. (2013). Taking the epistemic step: Toward a model of on-line access to conversational implicatures. Cognition, 126, 423–440.
Breheny, R., Katsos, N., & Williams, J. (2006). Are generalised scalar implicatures generated by default? An online investigation into the role of context in generating pragmatic inferences. Cognition, 100, 434–463.
Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 6, 1482–1493.
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1978)
Brown, P. M., & Dell, G. S. (1987). Adapting production to comprehension: The explicit mention of instruments. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 441–472.
Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009a). Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 171–190.
Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009b). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 893–900.
Bucciarelli, M., Colle, L., & Bara, B. G. (2003). How children comprehend speech acts and communicative gestures. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 207–241.
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112, 337–342.
Buz, E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). Dynamically adapted context-specific hyper-articulation: Feedback from interlocutors affects speakers’ subsequent pronunciations. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 68–86.
Chaiken, M., Böhner, J., & Marler, P. (1993) Song acquisition in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris: a comparison of the songs of live-tutored, tape tutored, untutored, and wild-caught males. Animal Behaviour, 46, 1079–1090.
Chevalier, C., Wilson, D., Happe, F., & Noveck, I. (2010). Scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1104–1117.
Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22, 1–39.
Cohen, P. R. (1985). The pragmatics of referring and the modality of communication. Computational Linguistics, 10, 97–146.
Cousillas, H., George, I., Henry, L., Richard, J. P., & Hausberger, M. (2008). Linking social and vocal brains: Could social segregation prevent a proper development of a central auditory area in a female songbird? PLoS One, 3, e2194. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002194
De Neys, W., & Schaeken, W. (2007). When people are more logical under cognitive load: Dual task impact on scalar implicature. Experimental Psychology, 54, 128–133.
Degen, J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2016). Availability of alternatives and the processing of scalar implicatures: A visual world eye-tracking study. Cognitive Science, 40, 172–201.
Del Vento, A., Bavelas, J., Healing, S., MacLean, G., & Kirk, P. (2009). An experimental investigation of the dilemma of delivering bad news. Patient Education and Counseling, 77, 443–449.
Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition, 11, 159–184.
Dumotheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). On-line usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, 331–338.
Epley, N., Keysar, B., VanBoven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 327–339.
Ferreira, V. S., & Dell, G. S. (2000). Effect of ambiguity and lexical availability on syntactic and lexical production. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 296–340.
Ferreira, V. S., Slevc, L. R., & Rogers, E. S. (2005). How do speakers avoid ambiguous linguistic expressions? Cognition, 96, 263–284.
Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. A. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 58–93.
Garrod, S., & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27, 181–218.
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The social construction of the cultural mind. Interaction Studies, 6, 463–481.
Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The power of thinking without thinking. New York: Little, Brown.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 3, pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.
Grodner, D. J., & Sedivy, J. (2011). The effect of speaker-specific information on pragmatic inferences. In E. Gibson & N. Pearlmutter (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference, (pp. 239–272). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hanna, J. E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Science, 28, 105–115.
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.
Haywood, S. L., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (2005). Do speakers avoid ambiguity during dialogue? Psychological Science, 16, 362–366.
Heller, D., Grodner, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference. Cognition, 108, 831–836.
Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344, 1243091.
Holtgraves, T., & Yang, J. N. (1990). Politeness as universal: Cross-cultural perceptions of request strategies and inferences based on their use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 719–729.
Horton, W. S., & Keysar, B. (1996). When do speakers take into account common ground? Cognition, 59, 91–117.
Horton, W. S., & Gerrig, R. J. (2005). Conversational common ground and memory processes in language production. Discourse Processes, 40, 1–35.
Jaeger, T. F. (2010). Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 23–62.
Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11, 32–38.
Koenig, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2010). Twenty-four-month-olds’ sensitivity to the prior inaccuracy of the source. Developmental Psychology, 46, 815–882.
Koster-Hale, J., & Saxe, R. (2013). Functional neuroimaging of theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, M. Lombardo, England: Oxford University Press.
Kravtchenko, E. (2014). Predictability and syntactic production: Evidence from subject omission in Russian. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 785–790). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Kronmüller, E., & Barr, D. J. (2007). Perspective-free pragmatics: Broken precedents and the recovery-from-preemption hypotheses. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 436–455.
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120.
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 9096–9101.
Kurumada, C., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). Communicative efficiency in language production: Optional case-marking in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 152–178.
Lin, S., Keysar, B., & Epley, N. (2010). Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 551–556.
Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of semantic and grammatical context on the production and perception of speech. Language and Speech, 6, 172–187.
Lockridge, C. B., & Brennan, S. E. (2002). Addressees’ needs influence speakers’ early syntactic choices. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 550–557.
McKenna, P., & Tomasina, M. (2008). Schizophrenic speech: Making sense of bathroots and ponds that fall in doorways. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced. Developmental Psychology, 43, 309–317.
Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329–336.
Naughtin, C. K., Horne, K., Schneider, D., Venini, D., York, A., & Dux, P. E. (2017). Do implicit and explicit belief processing share neural substrates? Human Brain Mapping, 38, 4760–4772.
Nijhof, A. D., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2017). Spontaneous mentalizing in neurotypicals scoring high versus low on symptomatology of autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research, 258, 15–20.
Nilsen, E. S., & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between children’s communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 220–249.
Nilsen, E. S., Varghese, A., Xu, Z., & Fecica, A. (2015). Children with stronger executive functioning and fewer ADHD traits produce more effective referential statements. Cognitive Development, 36, 68–82.
Noveck, I. A. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition, 78, 165–188.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.
Pepperberg, I. M. (1997). Social influences on the acquisition of human-based codes in parrots and nonhuman primates. In C. T. Snowdon & M. Hausberger (Eds.), Social influences on vocal development (pp. 157–177). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–226.
Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J. P., & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.
Poirier, C., Henry, L., Mathelier, M., Lumineau, S., Cousillas, H., & Hausberger, M. (2004). Direct social contacts override auditory information in the song-learning process of starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 179–193.
Pyers, J. E., & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20, 805–812.
Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070.
Sacks, H., & Schlegoff, E. A. (1979). Two preferences in the organization of reference to persons in conversation and their interaction. In G. Psathas (Ed.), Everyday language: Studies in ethnomethodology (pp. 15–21). New York: Irvington.
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage, 19, 1835–1842.
Schick, B., deVillers, P., de Villers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, 376–396.
Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2014). Spontaneous false-belief processing in the human brain. NeuroImage, 101, 268–275.
Schulze, C., & Tomasello, M. (2015). 18-month-olds comprehend indirect communicative acts. Cognition, 136, 91–98.
Schulze, C., Grassman, S., & Tomasello, M. (2013). 3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect verbal communication. Child Development, 84, 2079–2093.
Schwappach, D. L. B., & Gehring, K. (2014). “Saying it without words”: A qualitative study of oncology staff’s experiences with speaking up about safety concerns. BMJ Open, 4, e004740.
Scott, R. M. (2017). Surprise! 20-month-old infants understand the emotional consequences of false beliefs. Cognition, 159, 33–47.
Searleman, A., & Carter, H. (1988). The effectiveness of different types of pragmatic implications found in commercials to mislead subjects. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2, 265–272.
Sedivy, J., Tanenhaus, M., Chambers, C., & Carlson, G. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109–147.
Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mindreading. Mind and Language, 17, 3–23.
Stiller, A. J., Goodman, N. D., & Frank, M. C. (2015). Ad-hoc implicature in preschool children. Language Learning and Development, 11, 176–190.
Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 55–71.
West, M. J., & King, A. P. (1990). Mozart’s starling. American Scientist, 78, 106–114.
Yu, A. C. L. (2010). Perceptual compensation is correlated with individuals’ “autistic” traits: Implications for models of sound change. PloS One, 5, e11950.
Chapter 13
Akkermans, D., Harzing, A. W., & van Witteloostuijn, A. (2010). Cultural accommodation and language priming. Management International Review, 50, 559–583.
Athanasopoulos, P., Dering, B., Wiggett, A., Kuipers, J. R., & Thierry, G. (2010). Perceptual shift in bilingualism: Brain potentials reveal plasticity in pre-attentive color perception. Cognition, 116, 437–443.
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Dussias, P. E. (2017). Bilingual experience shapes language processing: Evidence from code switching. Journal of Memory and Language, 95, 173–189.
Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Biber, D., & Hared, M. (1992). Dimensions of register variation in Somali. Language Variation and Change, 4, 41–75.
Bock, J. K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992) From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150–171.
Broersma, M., & De Bot, K. (2006). Triggered codeswitching: A corpus-based evaluation of the original triggering hypothesis and a new alternative. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 1–13.
Brown, A., & Gullberg, M. (2011). Bidirectional crosslinguistic influence in event conceptualization? Expressions of Path among Japanese learners of English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 79–94.
Chen, M. K. (2013) The effect of language on economic behavior: Evidence from savings rates, health behaviors, and retirement assets. American Economic Review, 103, 690–731.
Christiansen, M. H., Kelly, M. L., Shillcock, R. C., & Greenfield, K. (2010). Impaired artificial grammar learning in agrammatism. Cognition, 116, 382–393.
Culbertson, J., & Newport, E. L. (2015). Harmonic biases in child learners: In support of language universals. Cognition, 139, 71–82.
Culbertson, J., Smolensky, P., & Legendre, G. (2012). Learning biases predict a word order universal. Cognition, 122, 306–329.
Dediu, D. (2011). Are languages really independent from genes? If not, what would a genetic bias affecting language diversity look like? Human Biology, 83, 279–296.
Dediu, D., & Ladd., L. R. (2007). Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 10944–10949.
Deutsch, D., Dooley, K., Henthorn, T., & Head, B. (2009). Absolute pitch among students in an American conservatory: Association with tone language fluency. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 2398–2403.
Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., & Xu, H.-S. (2006). Absolute pitch among Chinese and American conservatory students: Prevalence differences and evidence for a speech-related critical period. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 719–722.
Deutscher, G. (2010). Through the language glass: Why the world looks different in other languages. New York: Henry Holt.
Donadio, R. (2011, January 22). Surreal: A soap opera starring Berlusconi. New York Times.
Dryer, M. S. (1992). The Greenbergian word order correlations. Language, 68, 81–138.
Dryer, M. S. (2013a). Order of subject, object and verb. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved January 27, 2014, from the WALS database at http://wals.info/chapter/81
Dryer, M. S. (2013b) Prefixing vs. suffixing in inflectional morphology. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved November 25, 2013, from the WALS database at http://wals.info/chapter/26
Dryer, M. S. (2013c). Relationship between the order of object and verb and the order of adposition and noun phrase. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved from the WALS database at http://wals.info/chapter/95
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473, 79–82.
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 101–116.
Ettlinger, M., Morgan-Short, K., Farett-Stutenberg, M., & Wong, P. C. (2016). The relationship between artificial and second language learning. Cognitive Science, 40, 822–847.
Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 321–335.
Fabbro, F., Skrap, M., & Aglioti, S. (2000). Pathological switching between languages after frontal lesions in a bilingual patient. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 68, 650–652.
Fausey, C. M., & Boroditsky, L. (2011). Who dunnit? Cross-linguistic differences in eye-witness memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 150–157.
Fedzechkina, M., Jager, T. F., & Newport, E. L. (2012). Language learners restructure their input to facilitate efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 17897–17902.
Fedzechkina, M., Newport, E. L., & Jaeger, T. F. (2017). Balancing effort and information transmission during language acquisition: Evidence from word order and case marking. Cognitive Science, 41, 416–446.
Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83, 49–79.
Gibson, E., Futrell, R., Jara-Ettinger, J., Mahowald, K., Bergen, L., Ratnasingam, S., . . . Conway, B. R. (2017). Color naming across languages reflects color use. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 10785–10790.
Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 489–494.
Gladstone, W. E. (1858). Studies on Homer and the Homeric age. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language (pp. 73–113). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Greene, R. L. (2011). You are what you speak: Grammar grouches, language laws and the politics of identity. New York: Delacorte Press.
Hockett, C. F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. New York: Macmillan.
Hudson Kam, C. L., & Newport, E. L. (2005). Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 1, 151–195.
Hudson Kam, C. L., & Newport, E. L. (2009). Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 30–66.
Hupp, J. M., Sloutsky, V. M., & Culicover, P. W. (2009). Evidence for a domain-general mechanism underlying the suffixation preference in language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 876–909.
Kalmár, I. (1985). Are there really no primitive languages? In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 148–166). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86, 65–79.
Kay, P., & Maffi, L. (2013.) Number of basic colour categories. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved November 26, 2013 from the WALS database at http://wals.info/chapter/133.
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Hoshino, N. (2014). Two languages in mind: Bilingualism as a tool to investigate language, cognition, and the brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 159–163.
Liberman, M. (2011). Annals of “No Word for X.” Language log. Retrieved from http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=2920
Lupyan, G., & Dale, R. (2010). Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PloS One, 5, e8559.
MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–16.
Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of sounds. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
McWhorter, J. H. (2001). The world’s simplest grammars are creole grammars. Linguistic Typology, 5, 125–166.
McWhorter, J. H. (2002). What happened to English? Diachronica, 19, 217–272.
McWhorter, J. (2011). What language is (and what it isn’t and what it could be). New York: Gotham Books.
Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25–40.
Miestamo, M. (2009). Implicational hierarchies and grammatical complexity. In G. Sampson, D. Gil, & P. Trudgill (Eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable (Studies in the Evolution of Language 13; pp. 80–97). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Misyak, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). On-line individual differences in statistical learning predict language processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 31.
Munnich, E., Landau, B., & Dosher, B. A. (2001). Spatial language and spatial representation: A cross-linguistic comparison. Cognition, 81, 71–207.
Myslín, M., & Levy, R. (2015). Code-switching and predictability of meaning in discourse. Language, 91, 871–905.
Okrand, M. (1992). The Klingon dictionary (2nd ed). New York: Pocket Books.
Pagel, M. (2000). The history, rate and pattern of world linguistic evolution. In C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & J. Hurford (Eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language (pp. 391–416). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J. C. (2008). Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108, 155–184.
Poplack, S. (1988). Contrasting patterns of code-switching in two communities. Codeswitching: Anthropological and Sociolinguistic Perspectives, 48, 215–244.
Roberts, G., & Fedzechkina, M. (2018). Social biases modulate the loss of redundant forms in the cultural evolution of language. Cognition, 171, 194–201.
Roberts, S., & Winters, J. (2013). Linguistic diversity and traffic accidents: Lessons from statistical studies of cultural traits. PLoS One, 8, e70902. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070902
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Padden, C. (2011). The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29, 503–543.
Sarkar, M., & Winer, L. (2006). Multilingual codeswitching in Quebec rap: Poetry, pragmatics and performativity. International Journal of Multilingualism, 3, 173–192.
Sautet, F. (2006). Is language a determinant of reform success? Retrieved from http://austrianeconomists.typepad.com/weblog/2006/12/is_language_a_d.html
Stam, G. (2015). Changes in thinking for speaking: A longitudinal case study. The Modern Language Journal, 99(Suppl. 1), 83–99.
Stromswold, K. (2001). The heritability of language: A review and metaanalysis of twin, adoption, and linkage studies. Language, 77, 647–723.
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B., & Kuipers, J. R. (2009). Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 4567–4570.
Thomason, S. G. (2001). Language contact: An introduction. Edinburgh, Scotland: Edinburgh University Press.
Trueswell, J. C., & Papafragou, A. (2010). Perceiving and remembering events cross-linguistically: Evidence from dual-task paradigms. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 64–82.
Twain, M. (1880). The awful German language. In A tramp abroad. Hartford, CT: American Publishing Company.
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 7780–7785.
Wray, A., & Grace, G. W. (2007). The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua, 117, 543–578.