Plate 1

Plate 1 Egocentric and allocentric attentive processing; major differences in their efficiencies

This view contrasts our top-down dorsal egocentric networks with those other networks representing our ventral allocentric, bottom-up pathways. Your vantage point is from a position behind the left hemisphere, looking at the lower end of the occipital lobes.

This person’s brain is shown gazing up and off to the left into quadrants of scenery. The items here are imaginary. The baby and the hammer are in the space down close to the person’s body. The scenery above and the tiger are off at a distance.

Starting at the top of the brain are the two modules of the dorsal, top-down attention system: the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the frontal eye field (FEF). They serve as the attentive vanguards linked with our subsequent sensory processing and goal-oriented executive behavior. Notice how they are overlapped by the upward trajectory of the upper parietal → frontal egocentric (E) system. This Self-referential system is shown as an arching red pathway that begins in the upper occipital region. Notice that a similar red color also surrounds the lower visual quadrants containing the baby (at left) and the hammer (at right). Why? To indicate that this dorsal, “northern” attention system could attend more efficiently—on a shorter path with a lesser wiring cost — to these lower visual quadrants. This enables our parietal lobe senses of touch and proprioception to handle easily such important items down close to our own body.

In contrast, our two other modules for cortical attention reside lower down over the outside of the brain. They are the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) and the regions of the inferior frontal cortex (IFC). During bottom-up attention, we activate these two modules of the ventral attention system chiefly on the right side of the brain. There, they can engage relatively easily the networks of allocentric processing nearby (A). The green color used to represent these lower temporal → frontal networks is also seen to surround the upper visual quadrants. Why?

This is to suggest the ways this lower (“southern”) pathway is poised globally to use its two different specialized systems of pattern recognition. These are based on our senses of vision and audition. Each serves both to identify items off at a distance from our body and to instantly infuse them with meaningful interpretations. The yellow FG in parenthesis points to this lower pathway’s inclusion of the left fusiform gyrus. This region, hidden on the undersurface of the temporal lobe, contributes to complex visual associations, including our sense of colors.

Plate 2

Plate 2 Lateralization of the color field to the left

After beginning as a thin haze lower down and centrally, and having then risen to infuse both sides of the visual fields symmetrically, this yellow-green hue now becomes more dense as it shifts over into the left half of the visual field. The white oval area below depicts the gap between partially closed eyelids through which external light is still entering.

Plate 3

Plate 3 Coalescence of green into the left upper quadrant

After 15 minutes or so, the left-sided greenish color becomes more saturated. As it tends to coalesce in the left upper quadrant of the visual field, its background luminosity increases.

Plate 4

Plate 4 Midline circular zone of pink-purple

This figure depicts the color that next tended to arise, since 1974, out of the initial thin grayish red-pink haze. During these early decades, this circular area of color occupied central midline locations as the open-eyed meditator was gazing down. Currently, this midline pink-purple hue usually precedes the phase shown in the next color plate, yet it might also return many minutes later to follow it.

Plate 5

Plate 5 Coalescence of pink-purple into the left upper quadrant

The pink-purple-colored area becomes more saturated and luminous as it coalesces up in the left upper quadrant. The left superior quadrant localizations (depicted here and in plate 3) occur whether the subject’s open-eyed gaze is directed down or straight-ahead, or is directed gently up at an angle of 30° into the dark field under partially closed eyelids.