CHAPTER 7

UNRAVELING THE COMPLEXITIES OF EMPATHY RESEARCH: A MULTI-LEVEL MODEL OF EMPATHY IN ORGANIZATIONS

Gerald F. Burch, Andrew A. Bennett, Ronald H. Humphrey, John H. Batchelor and Athena H. Cairo

ABSTRACT

Purpose – Empathy, or the process of feeling or knowing how another feels, is a critical component of social interactions, and may be of particular importance to organizational functioning. This chapter addresses a literature gap on empathy in organizational contexts by providing a review of empathy research in a management setting.

Methodology/approach – We integrate the developing field of empathy research and provide a conceptual framework built on Ashkanasy’s (2003) five levels of analysis in emotions research, emphasizing within-person, between-person, interpersonal, group-level, and organization-level processes.

Findings – Our model addresses the complaint that empathy definitions are not consistent by illustrating how the level of analysis alters the view of empathy’s role in organizations.

Research implications – This multi-level model of empathy provides a framework to identify gaps in the empathy literature and make recommendations for future research.

Practical implications – This new model of empathy will help practitioners use and understand empathy by providing a structure of how empathy is manifested in organizational settings.

Originality/value – The field of empathy research has been limited by inconsistent definitions and a lack of a model that outlines how empathy is used in organizations. This multi-level model of empathy provides the necessary framework for researchers and practitioners to advance the research and practice of empathy in organizations.

Keywords: Empathy; affect; emotion; empathy in organizations; emotions in organizations; empathy review

Empathy is often described as feeling the same emotion of another person or “putting yourself in someone else’s shoes” (Lazarus, 1991, 1999). It is so socially important that Ickes (1997, p. 2) claimed empathy “may be the second greatest achievement of which the mind is capable,” since it facilitates communication and establishes relational bonds. In spite of this importance, organization research has suffered from lapses in empathy research and from a fractured research stream.

Initial empathy studies began in the 1950s and 1960s (Tobolski & Kerr, 1952), but relatively little research was published in the three decades that followed. In 2000, an influential theoretical article by George (2000) sparked interest in emotional intelligence and related constructs such as empathy. This initial spark was fueled by a special issue of The Leadership Quarterly on emotions and leadership (Humphrey, 2002), which contained conceptual, experimental, and field-based articles on empathy (Kellett, Humphrey, & Sleeth, 2002; Wolff, Pescosolido, & Druskat, 2002). Since 2000, more than 50 articles have explored the effects of empathy on leadership (Sadri, Weber, & Gentry, 2011), work performance (Munro, Bore, & Powis, 2005), communication styles (Silvester, Patterson, Koczwara, & Ferguson, 2007), job interviews (Fox & Spector, 2000), organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) displays (Joireman, Kamdar, Daniels, & Duell, 2006; Wong & Law, 2002), employee well-being (Scott, Colquitt, Paddock, & Judge, 2010), company profits (Stein, Papadogiannis, Yip, & Sitarenios, 2009), and compassion in the workplace (Dutton & Workman, 2011; George, 2013; Rynes & Dutton, 2012). This explosion of empirical and theoretical research has generated new ideas, yet at the same time developed conflicting definitions (Bagozzi et al., 2013) and research streams that can potentially detract from the overall research being conducted on this important construct.

The main purpose of this chapter is to fill a literature gap by providing a review of empathy research in a management setting, thereby offering three contributions. First, we integrate the developing field of empathy research and provide a conceptual framework built on Ashkanasy’s (2003) five levels of analysis in emotions research. Second, we address the complaint that empathy definitions are not consistent (Bagozzi et al., 2013) by illustrating how the level of analysis alters the view of empathy’s role in organizations. And finally, we use the framework to identify gaps in the empathy literature and make recommendations for future research.

MULTI-LEVEL MODEL OF EMPATHY IN ORGANIZATIONS

In this section, we propose a multi-level model of empathy in organizations; we begin the discussion by looking at why such a model is necessary. Our starting point is the changing definition of empathy. In 1873, Robert Vischer coined the term Einfühlung (translated to “empathy”) to describe how a person “feels into” another’s emotion (Titchener, 1999). Philosopher Theodor Lipps altered this approach by theorizing about the “inner imitation” that occurs when one person feels an emotion similar to the emotion felt by another (Montag, Gallinat, & Heinz, 2008). These two approaches were merged in 1990 when Salovey and Mayer defined empathy as the ability to comprehend another’s feelings and to re-experience them oneself. In the decades that have passed, some researchers have chosen to research the affective processes that lead to empathy (Walter, 2012), others have focused on the cognitive processes (Collins, Lawrence, Troth, & Jordan, 2013; Stueber, 2012), while others have combined both (Batson, 2009). The result is a conflicting set of research streams where empathy is defined based on the affective or cognitive components. Emerging neuroscientific evidence (Decety, 2011) may help resolve some of this issue by confirming two distinct empathic mental processes: affective empathy and cognitive empathy (Cox et al., 2012). Affective empathy refers to feeling the emotions of others, whereas cognitive empathy refers to knowing, or considering, the emotions of others. Based on this review of definitions, we propose a conceptualization of empathy in organizational research as a mental (affective and cognitive) process that (1) involves feeling or knowing others’ emotions, and that (2) motivates prosocial behavioral actions (Derksen, Bensing, & Lagro-Janssen, 2013; Smith, 2006; Zaki, 2014) (3) based on the social and organizational context.

Using this conceptualization, it is easy to see that empathy affects many organization constructs ranging from the individual to the team to the organizational level. At one moment, empathy is analyzed at the observable behavior level within the person (Joireman, Kamdar, et al., 2006) and then at the group and company performance level (Roberge, 2013). Therefore, empathy may be defined and measured differently based on the level of analysis chosen by the researcher. We propose that understanding the role of empathy in organizations begins with placing current research into a multi-level model of empathy in organizations.

Ashkanasy (2003) identified five levels of analysis that are important for emotions research: within person, between persons, interpersonal, group, and organization-wide. This model was further developed by Ashkanasy and Jordan (2008) and was applied to leadership (Ashkanasy & Humphrey, 2011a) and organizational behavior (Ashkanasy & Humphrey, 2011b). We use this five-level framework to integrate the empathy research in management, and therefore become the first to use the five levels of analysis for a specific emotional response. More importantly, our integration of research using this five-level framework allows us to develop a multi-level model that provides a framework (Fig. 1) for understanding empathy within organizations and facilitates future research. We discuss each level of the model in the next section and offer the following proposition:

Proposition 1. The conceptualization and definition of empathy within organizations varies based on the level of analysis.

image

Fig. 1. Multi-Level Model of Empathy in Organizations. Note: This multi-level model was constructed by applying current empathy research in organizations to Ashkanasy’s (2003) five-level model of emotions in organizations.

LEVEL 1: WITHIN-PERSON

Researchers who study within-person variability in emotions often base their research on Affective Events Theory (AET) (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). According to AET, people’s emotions and moods are influenced both by their average emotional baseline and by affective events that occur in the workplace. When considering empathy, potentially empathic events may trigger automatic empathic responses. Thus feelings of empathy vary throughout the day according to workplace triggers, natural biorhythms, people’s empathic baseline, and self-regulation abilities. However, the resulting observable behavior may be masked by organization members’ ability to self-regulate, and self-regulation may influence the within-person variability in empathy. We review the research from management literature on empathy for these automatic responses and self-regulation to understand the within-person aspect of empathy.

Automatic Empathic Response

Affective empathy refers to feeling the emotions of others and has been described as an “other-oriented emotional response elicited by the congruent welfare of someone else” (Batson, 2009, p. 418). This affective component is an automatic, or “bottom-up,” process that occurs within individuals, meaning it occurs before conscious awareness and is a form of imitation. Research on the Mirror Neuron System (MNS) has demonstrated that certain visuomotor neurons in the premotor cortex of monkeys discharge both when a monkey does a particular action and when observing another doing a similar action (Di Pellegrino, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese & Rizzolatti, 1992; Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Gallese, 1996). In humans, there is some evidence of motor cortex activation in similar areas as mirror neuron regions in primates when viewing another person’s behavior (Kilner & Lemon, 2013). These automatic responses may arise when we attend to the emotional reactions of others, particularly those in pain or distress. Our physiological and affective state is often altered when we empathize with others (Vignemont & Singer, 2006) to include activation of brain structures that control the heart and regulate automatic states (Critchley, Wiens, Rothstein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004). Research in this area supports early definitions of empathy as a form of imitation (Montag et al., 2008; Preston & Hofelich, 2012).

Cognitive Empathy

Cognitive empathy refers to knowing the emotions of others. This is when a person is aware of sharing the same emotion as someone else while also being aware that they are not experiencing that emotion due to the same cause (Decety & Meyer, 2008). The cognitive process is a more controlled or “top-down” process. Management researchers have studied empathy as an individual difference or trait, an ability that can change over time and with training, and as a short-term state that is based on situational contexts. Recent investigations have shown that it is more likely that an individual can learn or improve cognitive empathy but affective empathy remains more stable and is a trait-like process (Dziobek et al., 2008). Cognitive empathy can vary at the within person level because people’s focus of attention can shift through the day, making them more or less aware of others’ feelings. Throughout the day, people may also vary in how much attention they pay to their own feelings of empathy. Studies on perspective-taking have shown that people’s empathic concern can vary according to instructions that vary their focus of attention (Batson, Eklund, Chermok, Hoyt, & Ortiz, 2007).

Behavioral Empathy

Behavioral empathy is an observable action deriving from the cognitive and affective components. Most frequently, this is considered prosocial or citizenship behavior, and most researchers explain prosocial behavior motivation using the empathy-altruism hypothesis (Batson et al., 1995). Some studies have shown that empathic behaviors come at a cost, meaning that an individual might sacrifice something of value to them (e.g., money, time) when behaving in this manner (Joireman, Daniels, George-Falvy, & Kamdar, 2006). An evolutionary perspective on this altruistic response is that humans engage in these behaviors to help the species because during these interactions, all humans are considered part of the in-group (Preston, 2013). Although some people may normally be high on empathy, the propensity to engage in empathic behaviors is likely to vary widely during the day due to environmental circumstances, fluctuations in moods, etc.

Self-Regulation

The final discussion point at this level is the degree of self-regulation that occurs within the person. We have placed the empathic response in the center of our model, in level 3, based on the interpersonal effects caused by the empathic response. However, empathic responses can begin at the within-person level, as individuals must regulate the extent to which they experience, repress, and display empathy. As discussed in the next section, a variety of personality traits and individual differences may influence the ability of people to regulate their within person variation in emotions. For example, emotional intelligence, an individual difference level 2 variable, influences the ability of people to regulate their within person emotional variability. Some models of emotional intelligence include empathy as one of the subscales that measure emotional intelligence. For example, the Emotional Competency Inventory (ECI) (Boyatzis & Goleman, 2002) includes empathy as an important subscale.

Proposition 2. At the within-person level of analysis, empathic events may trigger automatic empathic responses within the person, while self-regulation may limit the level of empathic displays.

LEVEL 2: BETWEEN PERSONS

Organizational research has shown that an individual’s empathic ability affects many management constructs. Fox and Spector (2000) found that empathy was an important predictor of job interview performance. Similarly, those scoring higher on self-reported empathy saw higher sales performance (Dawson & Pettijohn, 1992; Tobolski & Kerr, 1952), have increased perceptions of interpersonal and interactional justice when delivering bad news (Patient & Skarlicki, 2008), and show increased moral judgment (Lardén, Melin, Holst, & Långström, 2006) over those with lower self-reported empathy. We propose that these individual differences exist based on the individual’s traits, dispositional tendencies, and their considered behavior.

Individual Differences

Antecedents to individual differences in empathy include age, gender, and self-regulation. Affective empathy is shown to increase as individuals get older (Sze, Gyurak, Goodkind, & Levenson, 2012). This may be in part due to older adults having more formative experiences that allow them to associate with others’ feelings (Hall, Andrzejewski, & Yopchick, 2009). It may be difficult for a young man to empathize with an older man because they have never been in that situation (Katz, 1963). The opposite is most likely not true for the older man’s empathy for the younger man’s situation.

A second individual difference observed by researchers is gender. Pfeifer and Dapretto (2009) suggested that females, on average, might exhibit stronger MNS involvement based on their role in caretaking of the young. This is supported by the research that suggests that females, on average, are more empathic and less analytic than males (Baron-Cohen, 2002).

An individual’s emotional and cognitive regulatory ability is also shown to affect empathic responding (Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004; Taylor, Eisenberg, Spinrad, Eggum, & Sulik, 2013). These regulatory abilities are therefore part of considered behavior that results from an empathic event. Greater emotional regulatory ability can promote an empathic response compared to a personal distress response in the face of another’s distress (Eisenberg, 2010). However, if people are motivated to avoid feeling empathy (perhaps when they think their empathy will be costly or unhelpful), greater regulatory ability can also predict a suppression of affective empathy (Cameron & Payne, 2011).

Dispositional Tendencies

Davis (1980, 1983) identified four dispositional tendencies linked to empathy: empathic concern, personal distress, perspective-taking, and fantasizing. These tendencies refer to the individual’s preferred method of empathizing, and once again demonstrate the affective and cognitive nature of the construct. Empathic concern and personal distress align more closely with affective empathy where the individual is emotionally altered by the event. Similarly, perspective-taking and fantasizing are cognitive events where the individual consciously considers what it is like to be the other person. Research has shown that individual differences further affect dispositional tendencies.

Although social roles in Western nations tend to conceptualize women as more affectively empathic and socially sensitive (Wood & Eagly, 2002), empirical evidence of gender dissociations in empathic dispositional tendencies are mixed. Women often self-report higher trait and state empathic concern or distress compared to men; however, these differences in empathy may be due to social norms of emotional expression and engagement, or interpretation of prior experiences (Batson et al., 1996). More recently, neuroimaging techniques have shown that women do recruit more cortical mirror neuron areas when engaging in both self and other-focused perspective-taking, which suggests that regardless of whether or not these differences are socialized, they do seem to be reflected by divergent neural architecture (Schulte-Rüther, Markowitsch, Shah, Fink, & Piefke, 2008). In essence, women’s empathic responding seems to be more “bottom-up,” starting with the affective neural process then moving to a cognitive process, whereas men’s empathy is often more “top-down,” recruiting pathways of cognitive understanding to motivate a neural affective response (Derntl et al., 2010).

Proposition 3. Age, gender, emotional regulatory ability, and cognitive regulatory ability affect the individual’s experience and displays of empathy.

Proposition 4. The individual’s preferred method of empathizing will affect the level of affective empathic processing (empathic concern and personal distress) and cognitive empathic processing (perspective-taking and fantasizing).

LEVEL 3: INTERPERSONAL

The previous two levels of analysis addressed the empathic responses that were automatic (level 1) and discussed how they were influenced by individual differences (level 2). These levels are important to management researchers because they explain the empathic processes occurring within the person and how these processes are different between persons. However, a significant change in research occurs in level 3 where the empathic behavior is finally exhibited in a social setting, thereby potentially affecting others in the organization. At this level, the situation affects empathic behavior, and this behavior subsequently alters interpersonal relationships.

Situation

While individual differences and dispositional tendencies of empathy are relatively stable over time, their effects on empathic behavior are moderated by situational factors. One situational factor which has received considerable attention is the degree of similarity, or representational self-other overlap, between the empathizer and other person (Decety & Jackson, 2004; Preston & Hofelich, 2012). Chambers and Davis (2012) suggest that similarity acts as a cognitive heuristic for empathic motivation, meaning the greater the ease in which someone can imagine themselves in another’s place, the more likely they are to feel empathic concern or compassion for them if there are no constraints on ability or motivation. A second situational consideration is experience. Batson et al. (1996) demonstrated that people who had recently undergone an experience of anxious anticipation showed higher physiological empathic arousal toward another person in the same situation. However, those who have gone through similar emotional experiences may also underestimate the intensity to which another person feels that emotion (Campbell, O’Brien, Van Boven, Schwartz, & Ubel, 2014). Thus, experience may more positively impact emotional empathy than cognitive.

The previous two situational variables allude to a potential mental model where it is more acceptable to empathize with those that are seen as being similar, or where one has experience with the situation. In essence, individuals may empathize with those in the in-group and not empathize with the out-group (see Tajfel, 1982). The perception-action mechanism (PAM) theory of empathy asserts that witnessing emotional states in others activates neural regions related to feeling that emotion as well as those of perceiving it visually or audibly. This neural overlap may activate shared mental representations of oneself and the other, which facilitates affective resonance and accurate perspective-taking (Preston & de Waal, 2002; Preston & Hofelich, 2012). Some evidence for neural self-other overlap comes from functional imaging studies showing that seeing another’s pain activated neural regions associated with feeling pain in one’s own body. Individuals high in dispositional affective empathy showed greater activation of these regions during these situations (Morelli, Rameson, & Lieberman, 2014). In contrast, this effect is attenuated when the observed person is of a different social group, such as race (Azevedo et al., 2013). This effect may be in part due to two other situational factors: noticing the person’s need and valuing their welfare (Batson, 1991; Batson et al., 2007).

Research indicates that while it may be possible that altruistic motivation is often automatic (Zaki & Mitchell, 2013), one must notice a need for help in order for that motivation to be elicited as well as take personal responsibility to help (Darley & Latane, 1968; Dickert & Slovic, 2009). Valuing another person’s welfare provides the motivation to be vigilant to the other person’s nonverbal and situational cues and intuitively adopt his or her perspective (Batson et al., 2007). In line with this reasoning, halo factors such as perceived attractiveness also promote empathic behavior, ostensibly through motivating greater attention to and valuation of the person’s welfare (Müller, Van Leeuwen, Van Baaren, Bekkering, & Dijksterhuis, 2013).

Empathic Behavior

Empathic behaviors exist at the center of our model and represent the transition between individual cognitive and affective processes which results in observable actions. Empathic behaviors motivate responses to another’s emotional state and well-being, thereby demonstrating acceptance, cooperation, or willingness to help. The empathy-altruism hypothesis proposes that empathy focuses one’s goal upon the welfare of another person or group rather than on their own interests (Batson, 1991; Batson et al., 1995). Empathic motivation is resilient to personal costs, and individuals are often willing to sacrifice something of value to them (e.g., money, time) when behaving in this manner (Joireman, Daniels, et al., 2006).

Dozens of studies have found evidence that empathic concern acts as an altruistic, rather than egoistic, motivator for helping others in costly helping situations (Batson, 1991). Empathizing with others also results in the sharing of similar emotions. This type of communication may allow individuals to accept one another and develop a relationship of appreciation (Tiedens, 2000). Consequently, when we are experiencing an emotion, if others do not share our emotions and empathize with us, “we experience ourselves more as objects and less as persons” (Katz, 1963, p. 496). Empathy is therefore an important social construct that can create bonds between individuals or build barriers when empathic behavior is not presented at the appropriate times.

Proposition 5. Individual empathic displays become important social events at the interpersonal level since they become observable actions. These empathic displays are moderated by the situation and stand to strengthen interpersonal relationships.

Proposition 6. The degree of similarity, individual experience, and placement within the in-group or out-group will moderate the empathic behavior by the individual.

Proposition 7. Empathic displays strengthen interpersonal relationships. The absence of expected empathic displays constructs barriers to interpersonal relationships.

LEVEL 4: GROUPS AND TEAMS

Much of the emerging research in empathy focuses on leadership, perhaps due to the importance of establishing good leader-member relationships at the dyadic and team levels. However, empathy research in level 4 is grounded in the social context which is affected by leadership and the social goals set by groups and teams. We begin this discussion by reviewing the research that has been conducted on leadership effects.

Empathic behaviors can impact leader emergence and leader performance. For example, Kellett et al. (2002) found that empathy was one of two routes to leadership emergence in groups, and that some people emerge as leaders by displaying empathy while others emerged as leaders through complex task performance and through their intellectual abilities. In a follow-up study, these authors developed a measure of interactive empathy that is especially appropriate for leaders. They found that group members high on interactive empathy were more likely to emerge as leaders and that interactive empathy predicted both relations and task leadership (Kellett, Humphrey, & Sleeth, 2006). They also found that interactive empathy mediated the effect of other emotional abilities (i.e., the ability to identify others’ emotions and the ability to express one’s own emotions) on both types of leadership. Moreover, interactive empathy was a more important predictor of leadership emergence than cognitive intelligence and complex task performance. A later study also found that empathy is related to both task and relations leadership, and that these types of leadership behaviors in turn predicted the quality of leader-member relations (Mahsud, Yukl, & Prussia, 2010). Likewise, Wolff et al. (2002) found that, in self-managed teams, moment-to-moment empathic behaviors predicted team leader emergence. Empathy is also positively related to perceived transformational leadership, negatively related to laissez-faire leadership, and has no significant relationship with transactional leadership (Skinner & Spurgeon, 2005). Perhaps most convincing to businesses, Sadri et al. (2011) found that leaders in 38 different countries who were perceived by their subordinates to be high on empathy received higher performance ratings than those low on empathy.

The effects of empathy at the group and team level are also influenced by the social goals of the group and team. Although organizational leaders often set the emotional tone for an organization, there are also cases where emotions and empathic concern emanate from the bottom-up. Muller, Pfarrer, and Little (2014, p. 1) argued that “collective empathy” occurs when organization members collectively respond to the needs of others, “thereby affecting the likelihood, scale, and form of corporate philanthropy.” According to their theory, top executives tend to use a more rational and cognitive approach when making philanthropic decisions, but they can be influenced by the more affective empathic responses of their employees. Therefore, the social goals, or the social motives to engage or not engage with others’ emotions create rules for pushing individuals toward empathic behaviors (approach motives) or to pull them away (avoidance motives) from empathic actions (Zaki, 2014).

Together, social goals and leadership affect the overall social context where empathy is encouraged or discouraged at the group or team level. It is this social context that provides the background for considered empathic behaviors. The leader, group, or team gets to determine the value of empathic behaviors, thereby adopting an approach motive pushing individuals toward empathic displays or avoidance motives. Subsequently, social interactions with those that display empathy may result in more considered empathic behaviors by the entire group. In a sense, empathic behaviors may have an emotional contagion effect, since emotional contagion appears to be driven more from affective empathy than cognitive empathy (Nummenmaa, Hirvonen, Parkkola, & Hietanen, 2008). This subsequent increase in empathic display can result in increased group affect as evidenced by an increase in employees’ willingness to engage in OCBs. OCBs are “contributions to the maintenance and enhancement of the social and psychological context that supports task performance” (Organ, 1997, p. 91). These are behaviors that are not specifically required by one’s job but are performed for the overall good of one’s organization without recognition. Joireman, Kamdar, et al. (2006) empirically showed that individuals higher in empathy are more likely to engage in OCBs than those lower in empathy. Leaders who are keenly aware of others’ emotions and who re-experience their emotions in an empathic manner are likely to take action to alleviate employee problems. Managers with higher levels of self-reported empathy have employees that self-report fewer somatic complaints, higher levels of positive affect, and increased goal performance (Scott et al., 2010).

Proposition 8. Social goals and leadership affect the overall social context whereby empathy is either encouraged or discouraged at the group or team level.

LEVEL 5: ORGANIZATION-WIDE

According to the multi-level model of emotions in organizations (Ashkanasy, 2003; Ashkanasy & Humphrey, 2011a, 2011b), organizational cultures create emotional climates that reflect their culture and that control or influence emotional expressions. It is proposed that an empathic climate also exists based on organizational leadership and the organizational context, where empathic displays benefit the organization as a whole. Executives higher in trait-level empathy are more likely to lead a company to higher firm profits (Stein et al., 2009). Moreover, empathy may help organizations deal with a wide variety of other issues, such as the need to be inclusive and support diversity at multiple levels (Roberge, 2013).

Recent writings in management have also brought the concept of organizational compassion to the forefront (George, 2013). Issues like income inequality or power differences between levels of employees can affect empathic behaviors. This may be because an individual’s interpersonal power impacts how people perceive their social interactions with that person, and this power difference can enhance or impair affective empathy toward an individual as well as the perceiver’s empathic response (Bombari, Schmid Mast, Brosch, & Sander, 2013). Thus, organizations with structures or cultures that are compassionate have the potential to manage the organizational context and therefore the propensity of members to engage in empathic displays.

Proposition 9. Organizational leaders create empathic climates that influence the degree to which empathic displays are encouraged or discouraged within the organization.

DISCUSSION AND FUTURE RESEARCH

The previous discussion of our multi-level model of empathy in organizations has focused on each individual level. However, these levels exist simultaneously throughout the organization, thereby creating interactions among levels. Individual empathic events that occur within employees before they arrive at work have the potential of becoming an empathic behavior that is observed by another later that day. Therefore, level 1 events affect level 3 interpersonal interactions. It is this nested system where affective and cognitive empathic processes are influenced by individual differences, the social context, and the organizational context that lead to the decision to engage, or not engage, in empathic behaviors. This observation alone should help explain the complex nature of empathy in organizations and the vast array of definitions used by organization researchers.

We propose that future empathy research acknowledge the multi-level tendencies of empathy and identify the level of interaction being investigated as well as possible interactions with other levels. It is also suggested that research be conducted within levels, and between levels, to further strengthen the claims of a multi-level model. In particular, little research has been conducted at the group level where social goals may affect the social context and therefore lead to approach or avoidance of empathic displays. Similarly, organization-wide research has focused primarily on organizational leadership and has not investigated other factors that contribute to the organizational context. Constructs of ethics, sustainability, and social responsibility are certainly areas that could be investigated at this level. A final consideration for future research should be on developing measures that address the cognitive and affective components of empathic behaviors that are capable of determining the influence of the action based on the social or organizational context.

CONCLUSION

This review of empathy was intended to provide a comprehensive framework to explain the complex nature of empathy in organizations. We conceptualized empathy as an automatic affective neural process and a controlled cognitive neural process which influence the degree to which perceivers feel and know others’ emotions, and which can lead to prosocial behavior depending upon the social and organizational context. We also showed how the multi-level nature of empathy explains why empathy can be defined one way by one researcher, and another way by a second researcher. Finally, we provided a review of past research within our new multi-level framework to provide recommendations for future research. We propose that this review is especially timely based on increased pressures for organizations to conduct themselves in sustainable, prosocial ways. Research on empathy may significantly address the means by which organizations can reach these new goals.

REFERENCES

Ashkanasy, N. M. (2003). Emotions in organizations: A multi-level perspective. Research in Multi-level Issues, 2, 9–54.

Ashkanasy, N. M., & Humphrey, R. H. (2011a). A multi-level view of leadership and emotions: Leading with emotional labor. In A. Bryman, D. Collinson, K. Grint, B. Jackson, & M. Uhl-Bien (Eds.), Sage handbook of leadership (pp. 363–377). London: Sage.

Ashkanasy, N. M., & Humphrey, R. H. (2011b). Current emotion research in organizational behavior. Emotion Review, 3, 214–224.

Ashkanasy, N. M., & Jordan, P. J. (2008). A multi-level view of leadership and emotion. In R. H. Humphrey (Ed.), Affect and emotion: New directions in management theory and research (pp. 17–39). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

Azevedo, R. T., Macaluso, E., Avenanti, A., Santangelo, V., Cazzato, V., & Aglioti, S. M. (2013). Their pain is not our pain: Brain and autonomic correlates of empathic resonance with the pain of same and different race individuals. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 3168–3181.

Bagozzi, R. P., Verbeke, W. J. M. I., Dietvorst, R. C., Belschak, F. D., van den Berg, W. E., & Rietdijk, W. J. R. (2013). Theory of mind and empathic explanations of Machiavellianism: A neuroscience perspective. Journal of Management, 39, 1760–1798.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.

Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

Batson, C. D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Todd, R. M., Brummett, B. H., Shaw, L. L., & Aldeguer, C. M. R. (1995). Empathy and the collective good: Caring for one of the others in a social dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 619–631.

Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 65–74.

Batson, C. D., Sympson, S. C., Hindman, J. L., Decruz, P., Todd, R. M., Weeks, J. L., … Burns, C. T. (1996). “I’ve been there, too”: Effect on empathy of prior experience with a need. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 474–482.

Bombari, D., Schmid Mast, M., Brosch, T., & Sander, D. (2013). How interpersonal power affects empathic accuracy: Differential roles of mentalizing vs. mirroring? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–6.

Boyatzis, R. E., & Goleman, D. (2002). The emotional competency inventory. Boston, MA: The Hay Group.

Cameron, C. D., & Payne, B. K. (2011). Escaping affect: How motivated emotion regulation creates insensitivity to mass suffering. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 1–15.

Campbell, T., O’Brien, E., Van Boven, L., Schwartz, N., & Ubel, P. (2014). Too much experience: A desensitization bias in emotional perspective taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 272–285.

Chambers, J. R., & Davis, M. H. (2012). The role of the self in perspective-taking and empathy: Ease of self-simulation as a heuristic for inferring empathic feelings. Social Cognition, 30, 153–180.

Collins, A. L., Lawrence, S. A., Troth, A. C., & Jordan, P. J. (2013). Group affective tone: A review and future research directions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34, S43–S62.

Cox, C. L., Uddin, L. Q., Di Martino, A., Castellanos, F. X., Milham, M. P., & Kelly, C. (2012). The balance between feeling and knowing: Affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional dynamics. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 727–737.

Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rothstein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189–195.

Darley, J. M., & Latane, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 377–383.

Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.

Dawson, L. E., & Pettijohn, C. E. (1992). The effects of empathy on salesperson effectiveness. Psychology & Marketing, 9, 297–310.

Decety, J. (2011). Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Empathy Review, 3, 92–108.

Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100.

Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1053–1080.

Derksen, F., Bensing, J., & Lagro-Janssen, A. (2013). Effectiveness of empathy in general practice: A systematic review. British Journal of General Practice, 63, e76–e84.

Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Eickhoff, S., Kellermann, T., Falkenberg, D. I., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2010). Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities: Neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 67–82.

Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.

Dickert, S., & Slovic, P. (2009). Attentional mechanisms in the generation of sympathy. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 297–306.

Dutton, J. E., & Workman, K. M. (2011). Commentary on “Why compassion counts!”: Compassion as a generative force. Journal of Management Inquiry, 20, 402–406.

Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 464–473.

Eisenberg, N. (2010). Empathy-related responding: Links with self-regulation, moral judgment, and moral behavior. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Prosocial motives, emotions, and behavior: The better angels of our nature (pp. 129–148). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (2000). Relations of emotional intelligence, practical intelligence, general intelligence, and trait affectivity with interview outcomes: It’s not all just “G”. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 203–221.

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

George, J. M. (2000). Emotions and leadership: The role of Emotional Intelligence. Human Relations, 53, 1027–1055.

George, J. M. (2013). Compassion and capitalism: Implications for organizational studies. Journal of Management, 40, 5–15.

Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., & Yopchick, J. E. (2009). Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 149–180.

Humphrey, R. H. (2002). The many faces of emotional leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 13(5), 493–504.

Ickes, W. J. (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Joireman, J., Daniels, D., George-Falvy, J., & Kamdar, D. (2006). Organizational citizenship behaviors as a function of empathy, consideration of future consequences, and employee time horizon: An initial exploration using an in-basket simulation of OCBs. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2266–2292.

Joireman, J., Kamdar, D., Daniels, D., & Duell, B. (2006). Good citizens to the end? It depends: Empathy and concern with future consequences moderate the impact of a short-term time horizon on organizational citizenship behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1307–1320.

Katz, R. L. (1963). Empathy: Its nature and uses. London: The Free Press of Glencoe, Collier-MacMillan.

Kellett, J. B., Humphrey, R. H., & Sleeth, R. G. (2002). Empathy and complex task performance: Two routes to leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 13, 523–544.

Kellett, J. B., Humphrey, R. H., & Sleeth, R. G. (2006). Empathy and the emergence of task and relations leaders. The Leadership Quarterly, 17, 146–162.

Kilner, J. M., & Lemon, R. N. (2013). What we know currently about mirror neurons. Current Biology, 23, R1057–R1062.

Lardén, M., Melin, L., Holst, U., & Långström, N. (2006). Moral judgement, cognitive distortions and empathy in incarcerated delinquent and community control adolescents. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 453–462.

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Lazarus, R. S. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

Mahsud, R., Yukl, G., & Prussia, G. (2010). Leader empathy, ethical leadership, and relations-oriented behaviors as antecedents of leader-member exchange quality. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 25, 561–577.

Montag, C., Gallinat, J., & Heinz, A. (2008). Theodor Lipps and the concept of empathy: 1851–1914. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1261.

Morelli, S. A., Rameson, L. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2014). The neural components of empathy: Predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 39–47.

Muller, A. R., Pfarrer, M. D., & Little, L. M. (2014). A theory of collective empathy in corporate philanthropy decisions. Academy of Management Review, 39, 11–21.

Müller, B. C. N., Van Leeuwen, M. L., Van Baaren, R. B., Bekkering, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2013). Empathy is a beautiful thing: Empathy predicts imitation only for attractive others. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54, 401–406.

Munro, D., Bore, M., & Powis, D. (2005). Personality factors in professional ethical behaviour: Studies of empathy and narcissism. Australian Journal of Psychology, 57, 49–60.

Nummenmaa, L., Hirvonen, J., Parkkola, R., & Hietanen, J. K. (2008). Is emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for affective and cognitive empathy. NeuroImage, 43, 571–580.

Organ, D. W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: It’s construct clean-up time. Human Performance, 10, 85–97.

Patient, D. L., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2008). Increasing the interpersonal informational justice when communicating negative news: The role of the manager’s empathic concern and moral development. Journal of Management, 36, 555–578.

Pfeifer, J. H., & Dapretto, M. (2009). Mirror, mirror, in my mind. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 183–197). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Preston, S. D. (2013). The origins of altruism in offspring care. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1305–1341.

Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–72.

Preston, S. D., & Hofelich, A. J. (2012). The many faces of empathy: Parsing empathic phenomena through a proximate, dynamic-systems view of representing the other in the self. Emotion Review, 4, 24–33.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.

Roberge, M.-E. (2013). A multi-level conceptualization of empathy to explain how diversity increases group performance. International Journal of Business and Management, 8, 122–134.

Rynes, S. L., & Dutton, J. E. (2012). Care and compassion through an organizational lens: Opening up new possibilities. Academy of Management Review, 37, 503–523.

Sadri, G., Weber, T. J., & Gentry, W. A. (2011). Empathic emotion and leadership performance: An empirical analysis across 38 countries. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 818–830.

Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211.

Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2008). Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy. NeuroImage, 42, 393–403.

Scott, B. A., Colquitt, J. A., Paddock, E. L., & Judge, T. A. (2010). A daily investigation of the role of manager empathy on employee well-being. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 113, 127–140.

Silvester, J., Patterson, F., Koczwara, A., & Ferguson, E. (2007). “Trust me …”: Psychological and behavioral predictors of perceived physician empathy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 519–527.

Skinner, C., & Spurgeon, P. (2005). Valuing empathy and emotional intelligence in health leadership: A study of empathy, leadership behaviour and outcome effectiveness. Health Services Management Research, 18, 1–12.

Smith, A. (2006). Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. The Psychological Record, 56, 3–21.

Stein, S. J., Papadogiannis, P., Yip, J. A., & Sitarenios, G. (2009). Emotional intelligence of leaders: A profile of top executives. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 30, 87–101.

Stueber, K. R. (2012). Varieties of empathy, neuroscience and the narrativist challenge to the contemporary theory of mind debate. Emotion Review, 4, 55–63.

Sze, J. A., Gyurak, A., Goodkind, M. S., & Levenson, R. W. (2012). Greater emotional empathy and prosocial behavior in late life. Emotion, 12, 1129–1140.

Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39.

Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324.

Taylor, Z. E., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, N. D., & Sulik, M. J. (2013). The relations of ego-resiliency and emotion socialization to the development of empathy and prosocial behavior across early childhood. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 13, 822–831.

Tiedens, L. Z. (2000). Powerful emotions: The vicious cycle of social status positions and emotions. In N. M. Hartel, C. E. J. Hartel, & W. J. Zerbe (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory, and practice (pp. 71–81). Westport, CT: Quorum Books.

Titchener, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tobolski, F. P., & Kerr, W. A. (1952). Predictive value of the empathy test in automobile salesmanship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 36, 310–311.

Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when, and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441.

Walter, H. (2012). Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: Concepts, circuits, and genes. Emotion Review, 4, 9–17.

Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes, and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 18, pp. 1–74). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

Wolff, S. B., Pescosolido, A. T., & Druskat, V. U. (2002). Emotional intelligence as the basis of leadership emergence in self-managing teams. The Leadership Quarterly, 13, 505–522.

Wong, C.-S., & Law, K. S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. The Leadership Quarterly, 13, 243–274.

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699–727.

Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: A motivated account. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1608–1647.

Zaki, J., & Mitchell, J. P. (2013). Intuitive prosociality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 466–470.