Many of the world’s languages show similarities to each other. Similarities may be just coincidences, and linguists are not persuaded that languages have a historical connection until they find reasonably systematic evidence. For example, the fact that the English word ‘three’ sounds and looks somewhat like the Dutch drie and German drei is of itself no proof that English, Dutch and German are historically related. It is only when we find a pattern of correspondences that we can take languages to be related. One of the relevant patterns here is that many English words beginning with th have counterparts in Dutch and German with d:
English | Dutch | German |
---|---|---|
thank | danken | danken |
thin | dun | dünn |
think | denken | denken |
three | drie | drei |
through | door | durch |
thunder | donder | Donner |
To establish that English, Dutch and German are part of a wider Indo-European family of languages, we need to pursue such regular patterns further, noting for example that Latin has a corresponding t in words such as tenuis (thin), tres (three), and so on. A painstaking search for correspondences such as these lies behind the assumption that the Indo-European languages of Europe and South Asia constitute a related family.
Closer to Australia, research of a similar kind has established that a large number of Pacific and Southeast Asian languages form a family usually known as Austronesian. This family includes languages as far apart as Hawaiian, Fijian, Maori, Indonesian, Javanese and Malagasy. Note, for example, the correspondences in:
Linguists have not found evidence to demonstrate a historical relationship between Australian Aboriginal languages and languages outside Australia. Aboriginal languages show no special similarity to Austronesian languages, for instance. On the other hand, Aboriginal languages probably are all related to each other. Of course the languages do differ from each other quite substantially, just as English, Dutch and German do, or Indonesian, Javanese and Fijian, despite their historical connections.
Often the words of Australian languages — the vocabulary or lexicon, as linguists would say — are strikingly varied. There are a few examples of words that can be found in similar form over a large number of languages. The word for ‘hand’, for instance, is often mara, mala or maa, and the word for ‘you and I’ is ngali in many languages. But often vocabulary is surprisingly diverse. Even neighbouring languages that seem closely related in pronunciation and grammar may reveal differences in quite common words. For example, in Arrernte, spoken around Alice Springs, the word for ‘dog’ is kngulya. In the Western Desert languages to the west of Arrernte, the word is tjitutja or papa; in Warlpiri to the northwest of Arrernte, the word is maliki; and even in closely related languages to the east and northeast of Arrernte, the word is aringka or alika. Despite this lexical diversity, there is enough evidence, from pronunciation and grammar as well as from a small number of related words, to persuade most linguists to treat Australian Aboriginal languages as one large family.
There have been attempts to classify Australian — to establish groupings within the family, just as Indo-European languages are grouped into Indo-Iranian, Celtic, Germanic, Romance, Slavonic, and so on. But these attempts have not provided a useful and lasting classification, other than to divide languages spoken in the far north from the rest of the country. To be more precise, Australian languages other than those from the north of Western Australia and the north and northwest of the Northern Territory show considerable similarities in pronunciation and grammar. They are known as Pama-Nyungan, a term based on words for ‘man’ in the far north of Queensland (pama) and the southwest of Western Australia (nyunga) (see Map 5). The northern languages, usually referred to simply as non-Pama-Nyungan, not only differ from Pama-Nyungan but are also more diverse among themselves.
We have already referred to grammar several times and need to explain the term. In recent years, grammar has not figured prominently in many schools. Those who have had little or no instruction in grammar may assume that it is something technical and obscure, and even those who have been taught something called ‘grammar’ may have gained the impression that it is a matter of learning a set of terms (such as ‘noun’ and ‘transitive verb’) and mastering difficult rules governing whether to say ‘it is I’ or ‘it is me’, or ‘for you and I’ or ‘for you and me’.
Map 5: Distribution of Pama-Nyungan and non-Pama-Nyungan languages (from Blake 1988, 87).
It is true that ‘grammar’ can mean something like a grammar book or a set of grammatical rules, particularly rules that people will keep breaking unless they are firmly taught them. But there is another sense of the word that means something like ‘the way in which a language is organised’. In this second sense all of us have a command of grammar, even if we speak only one language and have never consciously learned any grammatical rules or terms.
This point is not always readily accepted in English-speaking countries, partly because of an educational tradition of concentrating on only some parts of the language. In speaking English, we all follow many ‘rules of grammar’ which rarely if ever attract much attention. For example, we all obey certain ‘rules’ of word order. Anyone who speaks English says:
Did you hear that huge explosion last night?
No one who grew up speaking English needs formal instruction in putting these words in the right order, nor is tempted to break the pattern by saying, for instance, ‘did hear you’ instead of ‘did you hear’ or ‘that explosion huge’ instead of ‘that huge explosion’. There are many other such patterns underlying our normal usage. Even this one example is sufficient to illustrate a pattern for asking a question (‘did you hear’ compared with ‘you did hear’ or ‘you heard’) and a pattern for certain kinds of processes and things (in English you can ask people whether they heard something, as you can ask whether they saw something, but you would be asking different questions if you asked whether they heard or read ‘about’ something).
These rules or patterns can be described as part of English grammar. They are part of how we express ourselves in English. Other languages may or may not have similar patterns. In some languages, such as Dutch and German, a word like ‘huge’ would precede ‘explosion’, as in English; in others, such as Spanish and Indonesian, the order would be the reverse. Few languages happen to have anything like the English question pattern represented by ‘did you hear?’; in many languages the pattern would simply be a reversal of the corresponding statement: ‘heard you?’. In fact this was once the pattern in English but it has been replaced. Only in old texts, such as the 1611 translation of the Bible, do we find patterns like:
Know ye what I have done to you?
Sayest thou this thing of thyself?
Why baptizest thou then?
where more modern equivalents would be:
Do you know what I have done to you?
Do you say this thing of yourself?
Why do you baptise then?
Grammar as taught in schools in the past often had little or nothing to say about patterns such as these. And in a way this was understandable since most of us learned the patterns quite unconsciously before going to school. What was taught as grammar was often directed towards understanding and learning the patterns of other languages, such as Latin and French. That was also understandable, given the importance of these languages in the educational system of the time. It was unfortunate, however, that English grammar tended to be judged in the light of Latin grammar: in some instances grammarians and teachers actually tried to make English conform to Latin patterns, in others they imposed rules for which there seems to have been very little justification at all, and in general they fostered the idea that you needed to learn special rules to be able to speak English properly — or more particularly to be able to produce elegant written English. Many people still have an uneasy feeling that the way they normally use English cannot be quite right and that they need to remember and apply artificial rules to their written English.
Australian Aborigines had no tradition of writing grammars or rule books. On the other hand, their languages certainly had grammar in the sense of organised and patterned expression. Some of the patterning seems similar enough to English, but there are surprises for those who might assume that every language should be like English.
For a first look at the grammar of one language, some simple sentences are given below in Guugu Yimidhirr, a Pama-Nyungan language spoken by the people with whom Captain Cook made contact in 1770 in the area we now call Cooktown in northern Queensland. The examples are based on Haviland’s account of the language (Haviland 1979).
ngayu buli | I fell down |
nyundu buli | you fell down |
yugu buli | the tree fell down |
gudaa buli | the dog fell down |
ngayu nhina nhaadhi | I saw you |
nyundu nganhi nhaadhi | you saw me |
ngayu yugu nhaadhi | I saw the tree |
Some features of these sentences are similar to English (ngayu precedes buli, just as T precedes ‘fell down’, for example) but there are also many differences. There is nothing corresponding to ‘the’, and the word order in the last three sentences differs from English (‘I you saw’, ‘you me saw’, ‘I tree saw’). Moreover, Guugu Yimidhirr has different forms of ‘you’ depending on whether you are doing something (‘you saw me’) or whether you are on the receiving end of the action (‘I saw you’). Note that English makes such a difference between T and ‘me’ but not between forms of ‘you’, whereas Guugu Yimidhirr makes a difference in both cases. The word for ‘tree’, however, remains the same in both Guugu Yimidhirr and English, whether it is doing something (‘the tree fell down’) or not (‘I saw the tree’).
Some more examples of Guugu Yimidhirr begin to reveal how different the patterning really is:
gudaangun nhina nhaadhi | the dog saw you |
gudaangun nganhi nhaadhi | the dog saw me |
nyundu bayan nhaadhi | you saw the house |
yugungun bayan dumbi | the tree smashed the house |
Here we find something that is quite unusual compared with the English patterning. To account for it, we need to introduce some grammatical concepts. Firstly we need to distinguish things such as dogs and houses and trees from words referring directly to persons such as ‘I’, ‘me’ and ‘you’. Using common grammatical terms we call the first ‘nouns’ and the second ‘personal pronouns’. Secondly, we need to distinguish actions or processes like falling, from those like seeing and smashing. One way of describing this difference is to say that the first type are simple actions in which someone or something does something, while the second type involves a further participant, someone or something on the receiving end of the action. Again, there are grammatical terms often used for these two types of action word: ‘intransitive verb’ for the first, where there is no recipient or goal; ‘transitive verb’ for the second, where the action does, so to speak, affect or pass on to a second participant. Here are some examples of intransitive and transitive processes in English:
intransitive | transitive |
---|---|
I fell down | I saw you |
the dog barked | the dog bit me |
the girl laughed | the girl hit the dog |
a strong wind was blowing | a strong wind blew the tree down |
With these terms at our disposal, we can say that in Guugu Yimidhirr, personal pronouns have the same form when they refer to the performer of an action, whether transitive or intransitive, but they have a different form as recipient or goal of a transitive action. On the other hand, nouns take the suffix -ngun only when they represent the performer of a transitive process, otherwise nouns have the same unsuffixed form, whether serving as doer of an intransitive process or goal of a transitive process.
If we look again at Guugu Yimidhirr sentences like:
ngayu buli | I fell down |
gudaa buli | the dog fell down |
gudaangun nganhi nhaadhi | the dog saw me |
ngayu gudaa nhaadhi | I saw the dog |
we can summarise the patterning of the personal pronoun and the noun in the following chart.
pronoun | noun | |
---|---|---|
performing a transitive process | ngayu | gudaangun |
performing an intransitive process | gudaa | |
undergoing a transitive process | nganhi |
Most Pama-Nyungan languages have the same general kind of patterning as Guugu Yimidhirr. Many have a suffix that marks a noun as the agent of a transitive process, like -ngun in Guugu Yimidhirr, usually referred to as the ‘ergative’ suffix. Some have a suffix that marks pronouns (and sometimes some nouns as well) as the goal of a transitive process, usually referred to as the ‘accusative’ suffix. Most languages have a variety of other suffixes, which mark nouns for other roles besides agent and goal. In Guugu Yimidhirr, the noun bayan (‘house’) can take suffixes like -bi and -nganh:
bayanbi | in the house |
bayannganh | from the house |
The different forms of a noun or pronoun, suffixed to signal roles such as agent or goal or location, are referred to as ‘cases’.
Here are a few more sentences from Guugu Yimidhirr:
yiyi bayan | this is a house |
yiyi yugu | this is a tree |
yugu munhi | the tree is black |
What may be surprising to English speakers here is that there is nothing at all in Guugu Yimidhirr corresponding to English ‘is’ (or ‘are’ or ‘was’, et cetera). In Guugu Yimidhirr — and, in general, in most Australian languages — we do not say ‘X is Y’ but simply ‘X Y’. Thus, a language like Guugu Yimidhirr makes a fairly radical difference among three types of process: (1) intransitive processes in which someone or something acts; (2) transitive processes in which an agent acts on a goal or recipient; and (3) processes of being in which there is actually no expression of a process at all, nothing like English ’is’ or ‘are’. English does not distinguish these types nearly as sharply. In English, for instance, there is no difference in form between the actor of an intransitive process and the agent of a transitive process. Moreover, by the very fact of having ‘is’ and ‘are’ English also tends to make ‘being’ seem more like other processes of ‘doing’.
To illustrate some points about ‘having’, or possession, we turn to another Australian language for variety. Alyawarra is spoken in Central Australia, northeast of Alice Springs, and is closely related to other languages in the area, the most famous of which is Arrernte. The first few examples below show some structural similarities with what we have seen in Guugu Yimidhirr, notably the absence of any word matching ‘am’ or ‘is’, different forms of the pronoun ‘I’ and ‘me’, and an ergative suffix to mark a noun serving as agent of a transitive process (in Guugu Yimidhirr the suffix was -ngun, in Alyawarra it is -ila; details of Alyawarra can be found in Yallop 1977).
ayinga ngayakwa | I am hungry |
aringka ngayakwa | the dog is hungry |
aringka apmpima | the dog is hot |
awiya apmpima | the boy is hot |
aringkila ayinha utnhika | the dog bit me |
awiyila aringka atuka | the boy hit the dog |
The part of the grammar that we are now going to look at concerns the expression of possession. If we want to say ‘my dog’, a word corresponding to ‘my’ is added after the noun; if we want to say ‘the boy’s dog’, the word for ‘boy’ takes a possessive suffix (like the English -’s) and again follows the noun ‘dog’:
aringka atjinha | my dog |
aringka awiyikinha | the boy’s dog |
Phrases like these can serve as actors and goals, in the same way as simple nouns. If the phrase is agent of a transitive process, the whole phrase takes the suffix -ila, just as a simple noun would:
aringka atjinha ngayakwa | my dog is hungry |
awiyila aringka atjinha atuka | the boy hit my dog |
aringka atjinhila awiya utnhika | my dog bit the boy |
What is strikingly different from English, however, is what happens with phrases like ‘the boy’s hand’ and ‘my hand’. Since ‘hand’ is iltja, we might expect the Alyawarra to be iltja awiyikinha and iltja atjinha. In fact, we find the language works as follows:
awiya iltja apmpima | the boy’s hand is hot |
ayinga iltja apmpima | my hand is hot |
aringkila awiya iltja utnhika | the dog bit the boy’s hand |
aringkila ayinha iltja utnhika | the dog bit my hand |
Literally, viewed through English, the Alyawarra wording is:
the boy the hand hot
I the hand hot
the dog bit the boy the hand
the dog bit me the hand
Here possession is not marked at all, but the two words are simply placed alongside each other. In other words, in Alyawarra you would not hold the boy’s hand, you would hold the boy the hand, as if you were holding both the boy and the hand. If we are willing to think this through from the Alyawarra perspective, it does not seem unreasonable. After all, my hand is not something that I possess in the same way that I possess my dog or my car. My hand is part of me. When a dog bites my hand it does not bite some separate object that happens to belong to me, it bites me. The Alyawarra wording expresses this as the dog biting both me and the hand. In general, Alyawarra uses these two different patterns to distinguish between possession and a part-whole relationship. Anything that is a part of something will not be treated as a possession but will be expressed by putting the two words alongside each other, exactly in the manner of awiya iltja.
To look at it in another way, we might ask what it would mean to the Alyawarra to use English-style constructions. In fact, you could say ‘my leg’ in Alyawarra, using the possessive construction, but it would imply either that my leg is somehow detached from me or that the leg is not part of me (as if you were, for instance, sharing the meat of a bird and claiming a leg — ‘that’s my leg’).
Many of the world’s languages are in fact more like Alyawarra than English in this respect. We might say that they project a narrow view of possession. Indeed, it may be fairer to say that English and some other European languages are unusual in the extent to which they allow the possessive construction to cover all sorts of relationships that are not really possessive at all. Having looked at how a language like Alyawarra works, we may wonder why English so readily accepts phrases like ‘my arm’, ‘my name’ and ‘my father’. (This question is taken up in the second point for discussion at the end of this chapter.)
Another area of grammar in which there are significant differences between Australian languages and English is that of questioning. As we mentioned earlier in this chapter, English has its own ways of distinguishing between statements and questions, for example:
statement | question |
---|---|
you heard me | did you hear me? |
Jenny likes coffee | does Jenny like coffee? |
Harry doesn’t like tea | doesn’t Harry like tea? |
In fact, English grammar allows for various kinds of question. In addition to what you might call a direct question, open to a ‘yes’ or ‘no’, we also have questions that seem to expect ’yes’ rather than ‘no’ or ‘no’ rather than ‘yes’, as well as the possibility of merely repeating something with questioning intonation to check whether we have heard correctly:
does Jenny like coffee? | (open to ‘yes’ or ‘no’) |
Jenny likes coffee, doesn’t she? | (expecting ‘yes’) |
Jenny doesn’t like coffee, does she? | (expecting ‘no’) |
Jenny likes coffee? | (‘is that what you said?’) |
The grammar of questions in Australian languages is, of course, different from English. Here are some examples of statements and questions from Walmajarri, a language spoken around Fitzroy Crossing in the north of Western Australia. (Basic information about the language also written ‘Walmatjari’, is in Hudson and Richards 1978; but point 4 for discussion at the end of this chapter gives more examples and you may find it useful to tackle that now, to work out what each element means, including for instance the -lu in palu and ngalu.)
yanku palu | they will go |
yanku ngalu | will they go? |
yani palu | they went |
yani pa | he or she went |
yani nga | did he or she go? |
yani pa manga | the girl went |
yani palu mangawarnti | the girls went |
yani ngalu mangawarnti | did the girls go? |
There is yet another type of question in many languages — the information-seeking question that contains a question word such as ‘who?’ or ‘where?’ or ‘when?’ In Walmajarri, the words corresponding to ‘where’ and ‘when’ are wanyjurla and nyangurla:
wanyjurla palu yani | where did they go? |
nyangurla palu yanku | when will they go? |
The word for ‘who’ is ngana. Like other nouns and pronouns in most Pama-Nyungan languages, ngana takes an ergative suffix when it is agent of a transitive process. In Walmajarri the relevant suffix is -ngu:
ngana pa pinya mangangu | who did the girl hit? |
nganangu pa pinya parri | who hit the boy? |
It seems that in most Australian languages words like ‘who?’ and ‘what?’ actually have an indefinite meaning, so that ‘who?’ could be reinterpreted as ‘someone or other, I don’t know who’, and ‘what?’ could be reinterpreted as ‘something or other, I don’t know what’. Thus, a speaker of an Australian language might appear to be asking:
who took this photo and sent it to my brother?
when a better translation into English might be:
someone took this photo and sent it to my brother.
Now, there are two ways of approaching this grammatical phenomenon. We could say, more or less as we have just said above, that an Australian word for ‘who?’ actually means both ‘who?’ (questioning) and ‘someone’ (indefinite, unspecified). But we should stop to ask whether this twofold meaning of the word is really a consequence of approaching the language via English. Suppose we try to forget the English distinction between ‘who?’ and ‘someone’ and ask what it means to speak a language that has no such distinction. It would mean — and we cannot entirely forget English grammar when we use English to talk about another language — that you don’t really ask questions that seek specific information but you reveal gaps in your own knowledge to allow other people to fill them in. In this connection, we could compare two styles of interacting. The first would include direct questioning, expecting straight answers, and a conversation in this style might go something like this:
Who took the photo? | -My sister |
Does she do a lot of photography? | -No, not a lot |
Where does she live? | -Oh, in Queensland |
The second style of interaction would not ask any direct questions but would, so to speak, allow the other person to supply information:
Someone took this photo | -Yes, my sister took it |
Perhaps she takes lots of photos | -Not so many really |
She maybe lives in South Australia | -She lives in Queensland |
There is a sense in which this second style makes no distinction between information-seeking questions and statements. It’s a style in which you leave it open to someone else to add information. Persons who are used to the first style are likely to find the second style far too casual and indirect, while those who are accustomed to the second style might find the first too demanding, even aggressive.
Suppose, at the very least, that Aboriginal languages make far less use of direct questioning than English does. Then consider how important questions and answers are in English-speaking Australia. They figure prominently in all kinds of education — children are expected at an early age to recognise and answer questions, especially questions asked by adults, even when it is quite obvious that the adult already knows the answer (teachers’ questions like ‘what do we call a shop where you buy meat?’ or ‘what is thirteen plus twelve?’, or parents’ questions like ‘did you draw this beautiful picture?’ or, in a sterner context, ‘who’s left their room in a mess again?’, and so on). Later in the educational process, students are expected to ask questions themselves and we place great value on learning to inquire. We even make games and television programs out of questioning — quizzes, and so on. But this is not a universal pattern of life. Once we have taken the basic step of recognising that languages may be fundamentally different in their grammar, it is not so difficult to see how a society might build quite differently, regarding information as something to be shared rather than pursued, developing conventions for inviting information without demanding it, and so on. Eades has written about this difference in styles of interaction (Eades 1991, see also Walsh 1991) and she brings out the practical relevance of it in chapter 13.
Some readers may be familiar with terms that are used in talking about the grammar of English or other European languages, and should not expect all of these terms to apply, or to apply in the same way, to Australian languages. For example, the term ‘preposition’ is used to describe English words like ‘in’, ‘at’ and ‘for’. But it should be clear from earlier mention of cases, that the Australian Aboriginal equivalent of a preposition plus a noun (for example, ‘in Darwin’ or ‘for mother’) is often a suffixed noun (that is, something like ‘Darwin-in’ or ‘motherfor’). Thus, especially in Pama-Nyungan languages with elaborate case systems, the term ‘preposition’ may not be needed at all.
We have already seen that terms like ‘noun’, ‘pronoun’ and ‘verb’ are relevant, even if the concepts are not quite as in English. Similarly the terms ‘adjective’ (for words like ‘big’, ‘heavy’ and ‘good’) and ‘demonstrative’ (for pointing words like ‘this’ and ‘that’) do prove useful in describing Australian languages. Again, there are significant differences as well. For example, the demonstratives of Australian languages are usually much more complex than those of English: there are often different words for ‘this (very near)’ and ‘this (not very near)’ or for ‘that (distant but still visible) and’ ‘that (not visible)’. The English distinction between demonstrative pronouns (‘that’) and demonstrative adverbs (‘there’) may also not apply. For instance, ‘there’ may be translated as ‘that’ with a locative case suffix: looking at it through English wording, you say ‘that-at’ rather than ‘there’.
In many Australian languages, there is no distinction between singular and plural nouns. In other words, all nouns behave like English ‘sheep’ or ‘deer’. But some languages, including many non-Pama-Nyungan, have complex systems of gender or noun class which may include number marking. One of the simpler examples is Tiwi, spoken on Bathurst and Melville Islands off the north coast of the Northern Territory. All nouns belong to one of two genders, masculine or feminine. For example, thangkinangki ‘white-breasted sea eagle’ is masculine and ngaringa ‘black cockatoo’ is feminine. But human nouns may have a masculine, feminine and plural form (where the plural is not marked as masculine or feminine):
Walsh gives details of a more elaborate noun class system in a non-Pama-Nyungan language (chapter 8).
Even in languages without noun class systems, number may be evident in other areas of the grammar. As we saw earlier in the Walmajarri examples, number may be marked within or next to the verb. In the following example both the -lu attached to the interrogative element nga and the -warnti suffix on the noun are signals of the plural:
yani ngalu mangawarnti | did the girls go? |
The personal pronouns of Australian languages are also marked for number, dual as well as singular and plural. Some languages also distinguish inclusive and exclusive first person, where an inclusive ‘we’ includes the addressee, and an exclusive ‘we’ doesn’t. Some examples from Warlpiri are given to clarify the meanings and conventional grammatical terminology:
ngaju | I | first person singular |
nyuntu | you | second person singular |
ngali | you & I | first person dual inclusive |
ngajarra | we two (excl. you) | first person dual exclusive |
nyumpala | you two | second person dual |
ngalipa | you & I & other(s) | first person plural inclusive |
nganimpa | we (3+, excl. you) | first person plural exclusive |
nyurrula | you (3+) | second person plural |
Third person forms (omitted from the Warlpiri examples above) correspond to English ‘she’, ‘he’ and ‘they’. In fact, third person pronouns are not as common in Australian languages as in English, and absence of any pronoun may be enough to signal third person. In other words, ‘told me’ may be equivalent to ‘he or she told me’. When a third person pronoun does appear, there is commonly a single form for both male and female. In other words, there is a single word corresponding to both ‘he’ and ‘she’. (Bundjalung, described by Sharpe in chapter 5 is an exception to this generalisation, as are some of the northern languages with noun class systems including masculine and feminine.)
Finally, verbs may be extremely complex in Australian languages. The verb itself may carry a variety of suffixes to signal such distinctions as present and past (not unlike English ‘walk’ and ‘walked’). The verb may also be accompanied by various auxiliary elements (again not unlike English, where elements such as ‘have’ and ‘will’ contribute to the total meaning of verb groups like ‘have walked’, ‘will walk’ and ‘will have walked’). We have already seen in Walmajarri that the element pa or nga following the verb distinguishes a statement from a question. For a quite different auxiliary system, see the following Warlpiri examples. (Note that both ngaju and -rna mean ‘I’: the suffix represents a kind of agreement with the independent pronoun and is attached to the verb auxiliary, or if there is no auxiliary, to the pronoun itself.)
ngajurna purlaja | I shouted, I have shouted |
ngaju lparna purlaja | I was shouting |
ngajurna purlami | I’ll shout, let me shout |
ngaju karna purlami | I’ll shout, I’ll be shouting |
In this chapter we have looked at just a few aspects of the grammar of Australian languages. In some ways grammar may seem to be merely a matter of how this language or that puts words together. But we have also seen that the grammar of a language contains fundamental patterns that may have a lot to do with how we view the world and how we think about reality. In particular, the assumptions of English-speaking Australians about such things as possession and questioning are not independent of the way we habitually express ourselves in language. We do not want to overstate or exaggerate this point, but neither do we want to overlook the role that language plays. The exercises below include some further opportunities to explore the questions raised in this chapter.
Not all of the points we have mentioned here are unique to Australia. The use of a single pronoun for both ‘she’ and ‘he’, for example, is found in many languages around the world, including languages as diverse as Hindi, Indonesian and Japanese. And a final reminder: there are numerous Australian languages, each with its own grammar, and although many of them show significant similarities, we should be very cautious about generalising across all of Australia. Subsequent chapters of this book will continue to demonstrate the rich variety.
1. The second section of this chapter (on ‘Grammar’) mentions grammatical rules which are sometimes considered important in elegant written English but which many people don’t understand or don’t obey. The particular example given was that of the use of ‘I’ and ‘me’, where the traditional rules are strongly influenced by Latin grammar. It is interesting in this regard that there are some similarities between Latin and Australian languages. We saw above that many Australian languages have different pronoun forms for actors and goals. For example in Guugu Yimidhirr:
ngayu nhina nhaadhi | I saw you |
nyundu nganhi nhaadhi | you saw me |
Latin had a similar pattern of different pronoun forms:
ego te amo | I love you |
tu me amas | you love me |
In both Latin and Guugu Yimidhirr you would use the ‘I’ form of the pronoun after a process of being (but remember that in Guugu Yimidhirr there is actually no process expressed). In Latin you would say (for example, pointing at a photograph) ‘this is I’, in Guugu Yimidhirr ‘this I’. English obviously has a different pattern here, since we say ‘this is me’, but there were attempts to make English follow the Latin pattern and many people still have an uneasy feeling that ‘this is I’ is really what they ought to say. Such has been the confusion on this point that some people now over-correct, using ‘I’ instead of ‘me’ in many instances where the traditional rules happily accepted ‘me’. For example, it was once usual to say ‘please inform my colleague or me’: Latin would have used the accusative form of ‘me’ here and traditional grammar therefore accepted the use of ‘me’. But some people now seem to feel it is ‘more correct’ to say ‘please inform my colleague or I’.
For discussion, here are some examples of modern colloquial use of ‘I’ and ‘me’:
I’ll be seeing you
I can’t tell you
Me? I won’t do it!
John and me will call round
You’ll tell me tomorrow?
You’ll tell John and me tomorrow?
As a first exercise, check what you know about Guugu Yimidhirr and see which of these occurrences of ‘I’ and ‘me’ would translate as ngayu and which as nganhi.
As a second exercise, try to work out what the pattern of modern colloquial English actually is — how can you explain the use of ‘I’ in ‘I won’t do it’ but ‘me’ in ‘John and me won’t do it’? (You may find you have to explain it by the position of the word rather than by the role which the word plays in the sentence; and if you have access to a French grammar you may find an interesting parallel in the French use of moi and je.)
A third point for discussion relates to the suggestion that a language like Guugu Yimidhirr makes a sharp distinction between a relationship of being something and an action of doing something. What is the relevance here of the modern English pattern of saying ‘this is me’ rather than ‘this is I’? Does the pattern help to make being more like doing (since ‘me’ seems to be the goal of an action)? You might like also to look at the following English examples, where it might be argued that ‘being’ is treated as ‘doing’ something:
What did you do yesterday?- I was at my brother’s place.
I know I’m being a nuisance, but I want to ask you a favour.
Will you be the spokesperson?
I’ll volunteer if you’ll be my supporter.
2. Review the discussion in the section about the grammatical treatment of possession (‘Being and Having in Australian Languages’). In the light of the grammar of Australian languages, is it fair to say that English projects a wideranging view of possession? Consider examples of the way in which we refer to owning our bodies, for instance in talking about donating one’s body to medical research. Or consider the way in which people to whom we are related may be treated as possessions in our language: ‘my friend’, ‘my father’, ‘my daughter’, and so on. Is this unconnected with our social behaviour and attitudes? Add other examples that might occur to you in this connection and discuss the implications for our view of the world. The point is not simply to praise or condemn the English language for being different from Australian languages, but rather to explore what social and cultural differences might be related to grammatical differences.
3. Pursuing the discussion in this chapter about questions and statements, think about the consequences of not distinguishing between ‘who took my radio?’ and ‘someone took my radio’, or between ‘what did you find?’ and ‘you found something’. Add other examples of your own, again with a view to exploring just what the differences might be between Aboriginal and English-speaking Australia. Here, too, the point is not to reach sweeping and oversimplified generalisations about black Australia or white Australia, but to consider how societies and languages can differ and how they might misunderstand each other.
4. See how much you can work out about Walmajarri grammar from the following:
yanku palu | they will go |
yanku ngalu | will they go? |
yani palu | they went |
yani ngalu | did they go? |
yani pa | he or she went |
yani nga | did he or she go? |
yani pa manga | the girl went |
yani nga manga | did the girl go? |
yani palu mangawarnti | the girls went |
yani ngalu mangawarnti | did the girls go? |
yani palu parriwarnti | the boys went |
yani ngalu parriwarnti | did the boys go? |
yani palu, payi | they went, didn’t they? |
yani pa manga, payi | the girl went, didn’t she? |
yani palu parriwarnti, payi | the boys went, didn’t they? |
yani palu | they went |
yani ngalu | did they go? |
yani pa | he went |
yanta | go! |
yantalu | go (plural) |
yanana pa | he is going |
yanku pa | he’ll go |
yanany pa | he regularly goes |
Blake, B. | |
1988 | Redefining Pama-Nyungan. In N. Evans and S. Johnson (eds), Aboriginal Linguistics 1, University of New England Press, Armidale, 1-90. |
Eades, D. | |
1991 | Communicative Strategies in Aboriginal English. In S. Romaine (ed), Language in Australia, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 84-93. |
Haviland, J. | |
1979 | How to Talk to Your Brother-in-law in Guugu Yimidhirr. In T. Shopen (ed), Languages and Their Speakers, Winthrop, Cambridge MA, 160-239. |
Hudson, J. and E. Richards | |
1978 | The Walmatjari: An Introdecction, to the Language and Culture, Summer Institute of Linguistics, Darwin. |
Walsh, M. | |
1991 | Conversational Styles and Intercultural Communication: An Example from Northern Australia, Australian Journal of Communication 18(1),1-12. |
Yallop, C. | |
1977 | Alyawarra: An Aboriginal Language of Central Australia, Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies, Canberra. |