Chapter 33. Public Policy
Rune Elvik
Institute of Transport Economics, Oslo, Norway
This chapter reviews the contribution that traffic psychology can make to public policy designed to improve road safety. The stages of policy development in which traffic psychology can contribute are identified. The most important contributions of traffic psychology to public policy include (1) identifying road user behavior that contributes to accidents and developing ways to influence such behavior; (2) promoting the use of an experimental study design when evaluating the effects of road safety measures; (3) helping develop designs of vehicles, roads, and traffic control devices that are adapted to human capabilities; (4) predicting if behavioral adaptation to new safety measures is likely to occur or not; and (5) acting to correct a tendency toward wishful thinking that may develop among policy makers who may base policy on an overly idealistic approach to how road safety can be improved. Examples are given of all these types of contributions of traffic psychology.

1. An Analytic Model of Policy Making

Figure 33.1 shows an analytic model of highway safety policy making (Elvik & Veisten, 2005). The model is not intended as a description of actual policy making. It is a purely analytical model intended as a logical framework for identifying the types of reasoning and activities that constitute policy making. The stages identified by the model form a logical sequence; they should not be interpreted as a chronological ordering.
The first stage of policy development is to find out what the problem is and identify factors that contribute to it. In short, what are the most important highway safety problems and what are the most important factors contributing to these problems? The next stage is to develop targets for improving safety and decide on whether these targets should be quantified or not. Once the ambitions for improving safety have been defined, a broad survey of potentially effective safety measures (stage 3) is needed to identify those measures that can make the largest contribution to reducing the number of fatalities and injuries. However, for various reasons, it may not be possible to introduce all effective safety measures; an explicit consideration of constraints on safety policy can help in developing realistic policy options (stage 4). There will very often be more than one safety measure that can address a given safety problem; hence, developing alternative policy options that can be compared is instructive (stage 5).
A key activity in policy development is to estimate the expected effects of safety measures on the number of accidents or the number of killed or injured road users (stage 6). These estimates should ideally be based on the best available knowledge regarding the effects on safety of various measures. Any prediction (i.e., prior estimate) of the safety effects of a program will be uncertain, and it may be useful to explicitly consider sources of uncertainty and how to reduce uncertainty (stage 7). As already mentioned, policy is always made within constraints that are not necessarily chosen or wanted by policy makers; usually, therefore, several considerations are relevant for policy choice, requiring complex trade-offs (stage 8). Once it has been decided to implement a set of safety measures, the effects of these measures should be evaluated in order to increase knowledge of their effects for use in future policy making (stage 9).
Traffic psychology is not equally relevant at all stages of policy making. It can contribute in particular at stages 1–3, 6, and 9. A brief review of the potential contribution of traffic psychology to policy making follows.

2. Outline of the Potential Contribution of Traffic Psychology to Policy Making

2.1. Unsafe Road User Behavior as a Road Safety Problem (Stage 1 of Policy Making)

Road accidents are influenced by many factors. One of the most important is unsafe road user behavior. This includes speeding, drinking and driving, not wearing protective devices, talking on cell phones while driving, and a host of other forms of behavior. No study has assessed the contribution of all types of unsafe road user behavior to accidents or injuries. However, Elvik (2010a) tried to assess the risk attributable to 15 different violations of road traffic law in Norway. Table 33.1 reproduces the estimates of the risk attributable to these violations. These estimates are highly uncertain, but it is not possible to estimate statistically the uncertainty of each of the estimates. Confidence intervals are therefore not provided.
TABLE 33.1 Risks Attributable to Violations of Road Traffic Law in Norway
Source: Data from Elvik (2010a).
ViolationEstimate of attributable risk (proportion) with respect to fatalities and injured road users: Sorted by contribution to fatalities
FatalitiesInjured road users
Speeding0.2300.094
Drinking and driving0.1660.034
Not wearing seat belts0.1330.032
Health problems in drivers0.0930.080
Use of illicit drugs and driving0.0720.027
Service and resting hours0.0500.022
Not yielding at intersections0.0380.038
Not yielding to pedestrians0.0260.025
Use of cell phone0.0240.024
Red light running0.0190.019
Illegal overtaking0.0100.003
Engine tuning of motorcycles0.0060.007
Short following distance0.0020.012
Lack of child restraints in cars0.0020.001
Non-use of daytime running lights0.0020.002
Attributable risk shows the potential reduction of the number of fatalities or injured road users if the violation is eliminated—that is, replaced by driving that complies with the law. It is estimated as follows (Rothman & Greenland, 1998):
(1)
B9780123819840100335/si1.gif is missing
where PE denotes the proportion of exposure for which the risk factor is present—for example, the proportion of vehicles exceeding the speed limit. RR is the relative risk associated with a violation—for example, it is 2 if risk is doubled. If a violation represents 20% of traffic and doubles risk, the risk attributable to it is 0.167. This means that by eliminating the risk factor, the number of accidents can be reduced by 16.7%, given an unchanged amount of travel.
It does not make sense to add the estimates of attributable risk presented in Table 33.1. To estimate the potential for improving safety by eliminating all the violations, one can apply what has been termed the “method of common residuals” (Elvik, 2009a). The residual of an estimate of attributable risk is its complementary value—that is, the share of fatalities or injured road users not eliminated by eliminating the risk factor. Thus, for speeding, the residual with respect to fatalities is 10.230=0.770. By applying the method of common residuals, it can be estimated that by eliminating the violations listed in Table 33.1, the number of fatalities can be reduced by 61% and the number of injured road users reduced by 35%. For fatalities, the estimate is
B9780123819840100335/si2.gif is missing
These estimates are probably too optimistic because violations tend to be correlated. A more conservative version of the method of common residuals, which attempts to account for correlations, suggests that eliminating the violations listed in Table 33.1 can reduce fatalities by 52% and injuries by 32%. For fatalities, this was estimated as
2. Estimating the risk associated with unsafe road user behavior, thus providing knowledge about factors contributing to accidents and the size of their contributions.
3. Studying why unsafe road user behavior is widespread: What are the motivations underlying this behavior? Can unsafe behavior be reasonably modeled as (subjectively) rational from the road users’ point of view? If road users behave unsafely for reasons they think are good, does this imply that efforts designed to modify behavior will be ineffective?
4. To what extent can unsafe road user behavior be influenced by means of technical solutions that make such behavior impossible or unpleasant?
These are just some of the questions that are relevant for policy development.

2.2. Developing Targets That Are Motivating (Stage 2)

Many countries have developed national safety programs that are based on a quantified target for improving road safety (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), 2008). International bodies, such as the OECD, recommend setting quantified targets for improving safety. However, setting targets that will motivate both public bodies and others that influence highway safety to make an extra effort involves a number of complexities (Elvik, 2008):
To set ambitious but challenging targets, it helps to know what is the potential for improving safety by introducing various safety measures. A so-called “bottom-up” approach for setting targets derives a “realistic” target by adding up the estimated effects on safety of a number of safety measures that can be implemented. A “top-down” approach, on the other hand, approaches target setting from a more idealistic point of view. In practice, good targets involve a mixture of idealism and realism.

2.3. Surveying Potentially Effective Highway Safety Measures (Stage 3)

Many measures may contribute to improving road safety. A comprehensive overview of such measures can be found in The Handbook of Road Safety Measures (Elvik, Høye, Vaa, & Sørensen, 2009), which describes a total of 128 measures addressing the following elements of the transport system:
1. Highway design (20 measures)
2. Highway maintenance (9 measures)
3. Traffic control (22 measures)
4. Vehicle design, safety standards, and protective devices (29 measures)
5. Vehicle inspection (4 measures)
6. Driver training and regulation of professional driving (12 measures)
7. Public education and information (3 measures)
8. Police enforcement and sanctions (13 measures)
9. Post accident care (3 measures)
10. General-purpose policy instruments (13 measures)
Traffic psychology tends to be given blame or credit for safety measures that are directed at behavioral factors, such as driver training, information campaigns, or police enforcement. It is correct that traffic psychology has been involved in developing many of these measures, but it is a misconception to think that traffic psychology does not contribute to measures involving the technical components of the system. Knowledge produced by human factors experts regarding, for example, reaction times, cognitive capacity, visual performance, ergonomics, and many other specialties, has contributed importantly to current design standards for highways, traffic control devices, and automobiles. A freeway, for example, has been designed to minimize the task demands on drivers. It has no access points to properties along the road. There are no at-grade intersections. There are no surprising, sharp curves or steep hills. Pedestrians and cyclists are not permitted to use freeways. The road surface is smooth. Oncoming traffic is separated by a median. The risk involved in striking fixed obstacles has been reduced by impact attenuators. In short, a freeway is the type of road a psychologist might want to design in order to make driving as simple as possible and thus minimize the probability of errors being made. The effects on safety of measures targeted at road user performance and behavior are discussed more extensively in Section 3 (see also Chapter 16 for a focus on human factors).

2.4. Estimating the Expected Effects of Safety Measures (Stage 6)

The Handbook of Road Safety Measures (Elvik et al., 2009) contains a wealth of information regarding the effects of road safety measures. However, a mechanical and uncritical use of the book is not recommended when developing road safety policy and estimating the effects of road safety measures. There are three main problems:
1. The Handbook of Road Safety Measures often states only an average effect of a measure, although the effect can reasonably be assumed to vary systematically, depending, for example, on characteristics of the measure.
2. The quality of studies that have evaluated the effects of a measure may vary, and a summary estimate of effect should be based on the best studies.
3. Not all measures have been evaluated with respect to their effect on accidents; in particular, this effect will be unknown, but has to be predicted, for new measures.
Traffic psychology can contribute in particular with respect to the second and third of these points. Psychology has a long tradition of experimental research, and psychologists have contributed to the development of comprehensive methods for assessing the quality of research (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). Any application of the results of road safety evaluation studies should rely on a critical assessment of the quality of this research because poorly designed studies tend to produce misleading estimates of the effects of road safety measures. This topic is discussed in greater detail in Section 3.
The effects of well-established road safety measures on accidents reflect the net impacts of all causal pathways generating these impacts. In particular, road user behavioral adaptation will be endogenous with respect to effects on accidents; the effects on accidents always capture the effects of any road user behavioral adaptation. In other words, there is no need to “adjust for” behavioral adaptation when predicting the effects of measures whose effects on accidents have been extensively evaluated. The fact that road users adapt behavior is nevertheless not unproblematic because it normally reduces, and may even eliminate, the intended safety effect of a measure.
This is different in the case of new road safety measures. To predict their effects on accidents, it is necessary to predict whether behavioral adaptation is likely to occur. A framework for analyzing and predicting the effects of road safety measures has been proposed by Elvik (2004) and is shown in Figure 33.2.
A road safety measure will influence safety by modifying one or more basic risk factors that are associated with accidents. These risk factors include speed, mass, road surface friction, visibility, compatibility (differences in mass and crashworthiness between vehicles), complexity (the richness of information in a traffic environment), predictability (the accuracy of expectations), road user rationality, road user vulnerability, and system forgiveness (the safety margins built into the system). Changes in these risk factors influence the structural safety margin—that is, the safety margin built into roads and vehicles. These changes are sometimes referred to as the “engineering effect” of a road safety measure (Evans, 1985). The effect of a road safety measure on accidents, however, is also determined by the behavioral adaptation it may elicit.
Behavioral adaptation is sometimes in response to the risk factors a road safety measure is intended to influence, but it takes place before the measure is introduced. In Figure 33.2, this kind of behavioral adaptation is referred to as antecedent behavioral adaptation. As an example, drivers may adapt behavior to the technical condition of their cars. Technical defects may therefore not increase the risk of accident; once these defects are repaired following periodic motor vehicle inspection, drivers adapt behavior again, knowing that the car is in good technical condition. The net result could be that periodic motor vehicle inspection has no effect on accidents. Behavioral adaptation will sometimes also be the result of a safety measure, particularly if the measure is easily noticed, is associated with a large engineering effect, and road users can obtain an advantage by changing behavior (Amundsen and Bjørnskau, 2003 and Bjørnskau, 1994).
Will new safety measures, such as intelligent speed adaptation (ISA), intelligent cruise control, lane departure warning, or fatigue monitoring, lead to behavioral adaptation? ISA is a system that supports the driver in complying with speed limits. There are several versions of the system; one of them makes exceeding the speed limit impossible by regulating fuel supply to prevent acceleration to a speed higher than the speed limit. Because speeding is known to be an important risk factor for accidents and injuries (Elvik, 2009b), ISA would seem to be a potentially effective road safety measure. However, will drivers adapt their behavior to ISA? One common form of behavioral adaptation, increasing speed, is blocked by the system. Drivers could, however, adapt by becoming less alert. Some maneuvers, such as overtaking, might require more time and thus become more risky. Speed is such a powerful risk factor that it is difficult to believe that behavioral adaptation would entirely eliminate the effects of ISA, but it could reduce them.

2.5. Evaluating the Effects of Road Safety Measures (Stage 9)

To continue to improve highway safety, it is important to evaluate the effects of as many safety measures as possible. With its long tradition of experimental research, traffic psychology can make a key contribution to evaluation by helping to design experimental evaluation studies. There are few such studies (Elvik, 1998), but if the huge advantages of randomized, controlled trials were more widely recognized, road safety evaluation could become a more rigorous discipline, relying less on imperfectly controlled observational studies than it does today. Psychologists should regard it as one of their professional duties to advocate the use of randomized, controlled trials whenever they see a possibility for implementing this design. When experimental study designs cannot be implemented, researchers should opt for the best possible quasi-experimental design (Shadish et al., 2002).

3. The Scope for Improving Road Safety: An Overview and a Discussion of Some Measures

3.1. A Policy Analysis for Norway

Highway safety has been greatly improved in many highly motorized countries in the past 35–40 years (Elvik, 2010b). However, there is still potential for considerable improvement of highway safety. A policy analysis for Norway (Elvik, 2007) indicated that the number of road accident fatalities could be reduced by more than 50% by 2020 if all cost-effective road safety measures are fully implemented. The term “cost-effective” denotes a road safety measure whose benefits, according to cost–benefit analysis, are greater than its costs. In the road safety policy analysis made for Norway, four main options for road safety policy were developed. Table 33.2 shows the estimated effects on the expected number of fatalities of the main categories of safety measures that were included in each policy option.
TABLE 33.2 Potential Reduction of the Number of Road Accident Fatalities in Norway
Baseline values and main contributing factorsExpected annual number of road accident fatalities: Contribution of main categories of road safety measures to reducing fatalities
Policy option A: Optimal use of road safety measuresPolicy option B: Optimal use of measures controlled by the Norwegian governmentPolicy option C: Continue present policiesPolicy option D: Strengthen present policies
Baseline number of fatalities and forecast for 2020 (common to all policy options)
Mean 2003–2006250250250250
Expected in 2020 as a result of traffic growth285285285285
Reduction of the number of fatalities attributable to main categories of measures
Exogenous vehicle safety features49555855
New vehicle safety features42000
Road-related measures26283439
Enforcement-related measures2429343
New legislation4005
Road user-related measures2200
Total contribution of all measures14711495142
Expected in 2020 as a result of policy option138171190143
The mean annual number of fatalities during 2003–2006 was 250. In the baseline situation, involving no new safety measures but continued maintenance of measures already in use, the number of fatalities is expected to increase to 285 in 2020. These assumptions are common to all policy options. The following rows of the table show the estimated contributions of main categories of safety measures to reducing the number of road accident fatalities in Norway until 2020.
Exogenous vehicle safety features are those already on the market and whose use is expected to increase in the near future without government regulation. These include air bags, electronic stability control, seat belt reminders, enhanced neck injury protection, and high ratings in new car assessment programs. New vehicle safety features include ISA, intelligent cruise control, eCall (automatic crash notification), and event data recorders. Road-related measures consist of several large or small highway improvements, such as bypass roads, lighting, guardrails, and converting intersections to roundabouts. Enforcement includes both speed cameras and traditional enforcement performed by uniformed police officers. New legislation includes making bicycle helmets and pedestrian reflective devices mandatory. Road user-related measures are older driver retraining and stimulating more hours behind the wheel before licensing of young drivers.

3.2. Basic Driver Training

Elvik et al. (2009) reviewed and synthesized the results of 16 studies that evaluated the effects of basic driver training on accidents. Basic driver training refers to the formal training of car drivers before they are licensed for the first time. Depending on age limits, most drivers who are trained for the first time in their lives are 15–18 years old.
Figure 33.3 shows a funnel plot of 45 estimates of the effect of basic driver training on driver accident rates (accidents per million miles of driving). The abscissa shows estimates of effect; the ordinate shows the statistical weight of each estimate of effect. Statistical weight is based on the number of accidents: Estimates of effect based on a large number of accidents have more weight than estimates based on a small number of accidents. For a more detailed explanation, see Elvik et al. (2009). If estimates originate from the same theoretical population, their distribution should have a shape resembling a funnel turned upside down, with estimates based on small samples (at the bottom of the diagram) displaying a larger spread than estimates based on larger samples.
The summary estimate of effect is 0.97, corresponding to a small accident rate reduction of 3%. As can be seen from the diagram, a considerable number of estimates of effect indicate an increase in the accident rate. A closer examination of the studies shows that the effects attributed to driver training vary depending on study design. This is demonstrated in Figure 33.4, which shows mean percentage changes in accident rates in studies employing different study designs.
Study designs have been ordered from the strongest to the weakest. A favorable effect on accident rate has only been found in nonexperimental studies. Elvik et al. (2009) discuss various possible explanations of these findings. They conclude that methodological explanations are unlikely to be correct, given the fact that a number of experimental evaluations have been made. They conclude that the most likely explanation is that drivers adapt their behavior to their perceived skills. In other words, drivers who think they are good drivers may adopt smaller safety margins than drivers who are less confident about their own skills.

3.3. Graduated Driver Licensing

Graduated driver licensing (GDL) has been introduced as a means of making novice drivers understand that they are not yet mature drivers by restricting driving that involves enhanced risk, such as nighttime driving or carrying teenaged passengers. A large number of studies have evaluated the effects of GDL programs. Most of these studies report that GDL programs are associated with a reduction in the number of accidents (Elvik et al., 2009). However, there is evidence of publication bias, as tested by the trim-and-fill technique (Duval, 2005). Publication bias denotes a tendency not to publish research reports, for example, because the findings are not statistically significant or are regarded as anomalous, difficult to interpret or explain, or even unwanted. The trim-and-fill technique is a nonparametric statistical technique for detecting and adjusting for publication bias based on an analysis of funnel plots. The technique is based on the assumption that in the absence of publication bias, the data points in a funnel plot should be symmetrically distributed around the summary estimate. If there is asymmetry, this is taken to indicate publication bias, and symmetry is restored by adding data points that are presumably missing as a result of publication bias (Høye & Elvik, 2010).
The crude summary estimate of effect for all accidents is a reduction of 18%; adjusting for publication bias lowers this to 11%. For injury accidents, the bias appears to be even greater. The crude estimate is a 14% accident reduction; adjusted for publication bias, the accident reduction is 6%. Moreover, a tendency is seen for studies that do not control very well for potentially confounding factors to attribute larger effects to GDL than do studies that control better for potential confounding factors. Despite these reservations, the literature does indicate that GDL programs are associated with modest improvements in novice driver safety. However, the effects are far too small to eliminate the difference in accident rate between novice drivers and experienced drivers.

3.4. Speed Enforcement: An Accident Modification Function

The importance of speed enforcement should not be in doubt, given the fact that speeding is widespread and that the risk attributable to it is substantial. It is nevertheless clear that neither police officers nor speed cameras can be deployed at all locations and at all times. To apply speed enforcement optimally, two issues need to be resolved:
1. How is the effect of speed enforcement on accidents related to the amount of enforcement?
2. How should enforcement be carried out in order to maximize its effect in time and space?
With respect to the first of these issues, Elvik (2010a) developed an accident modification function for speed enforcement performed by uniformed police officers. Developing this function required considerable data editing and smoothing. Figure 33.5 shows the accident modification function.
A reduction of the amount of enforcement from a certain baseline level is associated with an increase in the number of accidents. An increase in the amount of enforcement is associated with a reduction of the number of accidents, but the marginal effect declines rapidly.
To maximize the effects of speed enforcement, the deployment of officers should be random—that is, the places and times targeted for enforcement should be selected at random—so that every driver should, in the long term, face the same probability of encountering the police (Bjørnskau & Elvik, 1992). The rationale behind this is that a random deployment of enforcement will prevent road users from detecting any systematic pattern in enforcement and adapt their behavior to this. Moreover, enforcement targeted at particular locations tends to be self-defeating in the long term: Once the police have successfully deterred most violators, there is a tendency for enforcement to be reduced. Violations may then return to the baseline level.
Regarding speed cameras, their effects tend to be very local (Ragnøy, 2002). To extend effects to a longer section of road, it may be necessary to electronically link several speed cameras and measure mean speed for the entire length of road covered by the linked cameras.

3.5. The Need for and Setting of Speed Limits

The need for enforcing speed limits would not exist if speed limits did not exist. Why not leave the choice of speed to drivers? Is there a need for speed limits? This question is discussed by Elvik (2010c), who argues that although most drivers probably think they choose the right speed and see no need to change it, the choices of speed likely to be made by drivers if speed limits did not exist would not produce optimal outcomes from a societal standpoint. Specifically, Elvik concludes that speed limits are needed for the following reasons:
In short, driver speed choice is not objectively rational—that is, it is not based on a correct assessment of all impacts of speeding, leading to a convergence of preferences regarding optimal speed. This does not mean driver speed choice cannot be reasonably modeled as subjectively rational—that is, as optimal given driver preferences and perceptions of the impacts of speed choice. A distinction between subjective and objective rationality is almost never made in modern analyses relying on the assumption that road user behavior is rational. This distinction, however, makes perfect sense with respect to speed choice.

3.6. The Prospects for Rewarding Safe Behavior—and Its Price Tag

In principle, a road pricing system can be designed to reward safe behavior by putting a price tag on, for example, speeding or tailgating (Elvik, 2010d). With such a system in place, drivers would soon discover that safe behavior brings a reward in the form of lower charges for using highways. A trial in Sweden, offering rewards for complying with speed limits, found that drivers do respond to economic incentives (Lindberg, 2006). However, many drivers would regard the system as an unacceptable invasion of privacy and might not perceive the lower charges associated with safe behavior as a reward because they would still be paying to use highways. An option that deserves consideration is to charge more for speeding than the societal cost it generates in order to make a surplus. This surplus could then be paid back to law-abiding drivers to make the reward for safe behavior more tangible.
Drivers can be provided with monetary incentives for safe behavior if they consent to having their behavior monitored continuously and in great detail by a driving computer and, possibly, a camera capturing their face. Because many drivers probably regard the current level of accident risk as perfectly acceptable, it is not very likely that they would see any advantage of introducing an invasive technology designed to discourage them from speeding, driving when fatigued, or committing simple errors such as forgetting to signal when turning.

4. Discussion and Summary

Highway safety has been greatly improved in many highly motorized countries in the past 40–50 years. The rate of progress has not been the same in all countries, but there is no doubt that highway travel is considerably safer today than it was when traffic fatalities peaked in the highly motorized countries in 1970–1972. What accounts for the improvement in highway safety? To what extent has knowledge gained in traffic psychology contributed to it? It is difficult to give very precise answers to these questions. The improvement in highway safety is no doubt the result of a large number of safety measures that have been introduced but probably also the result of less tangible factors, such as subtle changes in culture or a higher demand for and valuation of safety as a result of greater wealth.
Traffic psychology may, in a sense, be regarded as the dismal science of traffic safety. The term “dismal science” is usually reserved for economics because economists often remind us that resources are scarce, that we cannot get everything we want, that we are greedy and egocentric, that cycles of boom and bust will repeat themselves, and so on. Traffic psychology reminds us that humans are the most difficult part of the highway system to change. Road users will commit errors, misperceive risks, or deliberately take risks, such as drinking and driving, speeding, and so on. One is left with the impression that little can be done to change this. This impression is too pessimistic.
During the past 40–50 years, several important changes in road user behavior have contributed to improved safety. The wearing of seat belts has increased in all highly motorized countries. Children are more often restrained in cars than in the past. Drinking and driving has probably been reduced in many countries, although data confirming this are less complete than the data on seat belt wearing and the use of child restraints. In many motorized countries, more motorcycle riders wear helmets today than 40–50 years ago. In the United States, however, laws mandating the use of helmets by motorcyclists have remained controversial and have been repealed in many states.
Despite these improvements, unsafe road user behavior continues to be a major road safety problem. What are the prospects of significantly reducing the contribution that unsafe road user behavior makes to traffic fatalities? It depends on which measures are taken to influence road user behavior. Persuasion alone is not likely to be very effective. Most road users think that their behavior is entirely appropriate and see no reason to change it. Telling them to change is unlikely to impress them. Repression may be more effective. More police enforcement will contain speeding and other types of unsafe behavior, but it is impossible for the police to be everywhere at all times. The risk of apprehension will remain low. From a theoretical standpoint, rewarding safe behavior is the most attractive option for promoting it. However, to reward safe behavior, it is necessary to observe behavior in some detail, and the technology permitting such observation will probably be regarded as highly intrusive by many drivers. Drivers may reject this technology, although it could make travel much safer than it is today.