Abraham, R., and Shaw, C. (1982–1988). Dynamics: A Visual Introduction. Santa Cruz, CA: Ariel Press.
Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behavior. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 165–178.
Agre, P. E. (1995). Computational research on interaction and agency. Artificial Intelligence 72: 1–52.
Allman, J. M. (1999). Evolving Brains. New York: Scientific American Library.
Andersen, P. B., Emmeche, C., Finnemann, N. O., and Christiansen, P. V., eds. (2000). Downward Causation. Minds, Bodies and Matter. Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University Press.
Arendt, D., and Jung, K. N. (1994). Inversion of dorso ventral axis? Nature 371: 26.
Arthur, W. (2002). The emerging conceptual framework of evolutionary developmental biology. Nature 415: 757–764.
Atmanspacher, H., and Bishop, R., eds. (2002). Between Chance and Choice: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Determinism. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Austin, J. H. (1998). Zen and the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bachman, P. A., Luisi, P. L., and Lang, J. (1992). Autocatalytic selfreplicating micelles as models for prebiotic structures. Nature 357: 57–59.
Bach-y-Rita, P. (2002). Sensory substitution and qualia. In A. Noë and E. Thompson, eds., Vision and Mind: Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception, pp. 497–514. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Barresi, J., and Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19: 107–154.
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps toward an Ecology of Mind. New York: Ballantine Books.
Bayne, T. (2004). Closing the gap? Some questions for neurophenomenology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 3: 349–364.
Bechtel, W., and Richardson, R. C. (1993). Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and Localization as Strategies in Scientific Research. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Beer, R. D. (2000). Dynamical approaches to cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 91–98.
Belousek, D. W. (2003). Non-separability, non-supervenience, and quantum ontology. Philosophy of Science 70: 791–811.
Bermúdez, J. L. (1998). The Paradox of Self-Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bernet, R. (2002). Unconscious consciousness in Husserl and Freud. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 327–351.
Bernet, R., Kern, I., and Marbach, E. (1993). An Introduction to Husserlian Phenomenology. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Bishop, R. C. (2002). Downward causation in complex systems. British Society for the Philosophy of Science Annual Meeting, Glasgow, Scotland, 4–5 July 2002.
Bitbol, M. (2003). A cure for metaphysical illusions: Kant, quantum mechanics, and the Madhyamaka. In B. Alan Wallace, ed., Buddhism and Science: Breaking New Ground, pp. 325–361. New York: Columbia University Press.
———. (in press). Ontology, matter and emergence. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.
Bitbol, M., and Luisi, P. L. (2005). Autopoiesis with or without cognition: defining life at its edge. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 1: 99–107.
Blackmore, S. (2002). There is no stream of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 9: 17–28.
———. (2003). Consciousness: An Introduction. Oxon: Hodder & Stoughton.
Blackmore, S. J., Brelstaff, G., Nelson, K., and Troscianko, T. (1995). Is the richness of our visual world an illusion? Transaccadic memory for complex scenes. Perception 24: 1075–1081.
Blake, R. (2001). A primer on binocular rivalry, including current controversies. Brain and Mind 2: 5–38.
Blake, R., and Logothetis, N. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3: 1–11.
Block, N., ed. (1981a). Imagery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———, ed. (1981b). Readings in the Philosophy of Psychology, vol. 2. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
———. (1983). Mental pictures and cognitive science. Philosophical Review 93: 499–542.
———. (1997). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. In N. Block, O. Flanagan, and G. Güzeldere, eds., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates, pp. 375–416. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Boden, M. (2000). Autopoiesis and life. Cognitive Science Quarterly 1: 117–145.
Bonner, J. T. (1980). The Evolution of Culture in Animal Societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Botero, J.-J. (1999). The immediately given as ground and background. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 440–463. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Bourgine, P., and Stewart, J. (2004). Autopoiesis and cognition. Artificial Life 20: 327–345.
Bressler, S., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2001). Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 26–36.
Brockman, J. (1995). The Third Culture. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Brough, J. B. (1972). The emergence of an absolute consciousness in Husserl’s early writings on time-consciousness. Man and World 5: 298–326.
———. (1989). Husserl’s phenomenology of time-consciousness. In J. N. Mohanty and W. R. McKenna, eds., Husserl’s Phenomenology: A Textbook, pp. 249–289. Washington, DC: Center for Advanced Research in Phenomenology.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruzina, R. (2004). Phenomenology and cognitive science: moving beyond the paradigms. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 20: 43–48.
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., and Freund, H.-J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience 13: 400–404.
Burian, R. M., and Richardson, R. C. (1996). Form and order in evolutionary biology. In M. Boden, ed., The Philosophy of Artificial Life, pp. 146–172. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1996.
Buss, L. W. (1987). The Evolution of Individuality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Campbell, D. (1974). “Downward causation” in hierarchically organized biological systems. In F. J. Ayala and T. Dobzhansky, eds., Studies in the Philosophy of Biology, pp. 179–186. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Campbell, R. J., and Bickhard, M. H. (2002). Physicalism, emergence, and downward causation. Unpublished. URL: http://www.lehigh.edu/˜mhb0/physicalemergence.pdf
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C., and Lenzi, J. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100: 5497–5502.
Cartwright, N. (1997). Why physics? In R. Penrose, A. Shimony, N. Cartwright, and S. Hawking, The Large, the Small, and the Human Mind, pp. 161–168. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Casey, E. (2000). Imagining: A Phenomenological Study, 2d ed. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The Conscious Mind: In Search of a Fundamental Theory. New York: Oxford University Press.
———. (1997). Moving forward on the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4: 3–46.
———. (2000). What is a neural correlate of consciousness? In T. Metzinger, ed., Neural Correlates of Consciousness, pp. 18–39. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (2002). Consciousness and its place in nature. In D. J. Chalmers, ed., Philosophy of Mind. Classical and Contemporary Readings, pp. 247–272. New York: Oxford University Press.
Charland, L. C. (2001). In defence of “emotion”: Critical notice of Paul E. Griffiths, What Emotions Really Are: The Problem of Psychological Categories. Canadian Journal of Philosophy 31: 133–154.
Chiel, H., and Beer, R. (1997). The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment. Trends in Neurosciences 20: 553–557.
Church, J. (1997). Fallacies or analyses? In N. Block, O. Flanagan, and G. Güzeldere, eds., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates, pp. 425–426. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (2001). Mindware. An Introduction to the Philosophy of Cognitive Science. New York: Oxford University Press.
Clark, A., and Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis 58: 7–19.
Coen, E. (1999). The Art of the Genes: How Organisms Make Themselves. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cole, J. (1997). On being “faceless”: selfhood and facial embodiment. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4: 467–484.
Cole, J., Depraz, N., and Gallagher, S. (2000). Unity and disunity in bodily awareness: phenomenology and neuroscience. Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness IV. Workshop. Brussels. URL: http://pegasus.cc.ucf.edu/˜gallaghr/brus3.html
Colombetti, G. (2005). Appraising valence. In G. Colombetti and E. Thompson, eds., Emotion Experience, pp. 103–126. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 12: 103–126.
———. (in press). Enactive appraisal. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.
Colombetti, G., and Thompson, E., eds. (2005a). Emotion Experience. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published as a special issue of the Journal of Consciousness Studies 12(8–9).
———. (2005b). Enacting emotional interpretations with feeling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 200–201.
———. (2006). The feeling body: Toward an enactive approach to emotion. In W. Overton, U. Mueller, and J. Newman, eds., Body in Mind, Mind in Body: Developmental Perspectives on Embodiment and Consciousness. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Conway Morris, S. (1998). The Crucible of Creation: The Burgess Shale and the Rise of Animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cornell, J. F. (1986). A Newton of the grassblade? Darwin and the problem of organic teleology. Isis 77: 405–421.
Cosmelli, D., David, O., Lachaux, J.-P., Martinerie, J., Garnero, L., Renault, B., and Varela, F. J. (2004). Waves of consciousness: ongoing cortical patterns during binocular rivalry. Neuroimage 23: 128–140.
Cosmelli, D., Lachaux, J.-P., and Thompson, E. (2007). Neurodynamical approaches to consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson, eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cottrell, A. (1999). Sniffing the camembert: on the conceivability of zombies. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 4–12.
Coutinho, A. (2003). A walk with Francisco Varela from first to secondgeneration networks: in search of the structure, dynamics and metadynamics of an organism-centered immune system. Biological Research 36: 17–26.
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3: 655–666.
Crick, F. (1958). On protein synthesis. Symposium of the Society for Experimental Biology 12: 138–163.
———. (1994). The Astonishing Hypothesis: The Scientific Search for the Soul. New York: Scribner’s.
Crowell, S. (in press). Phenomenological immanence and semantic externalism: a rapprochement. Synthese, in press.
Dainton, B. (2000). Stream of Consciousness: Unity and Continuity in Conscious Experience. London: Routledge Press.
———. (2002). The gaze of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 9: 31–48.
Damasio, A. R. (1990). Synchronous activation in multiple cortical regions: a mechanism for recall. Seminars in the Neurosciences 2: 287–297.
———. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.
———. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. New York: Harcourt.
Darwin, C. (1996). On Evolution: The Development of the Theory of Natural Selection. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing Company.
David, O., Cosmelli, D., Hasboun, D., and Garnero, L. (2003). A multitrial analysis for revealing significant corticocortical networks in magentoencaphalography and electroencephalography. Neuroimage 20: 186–201.
David, O., Cosmelli, D., Lachaux, J.-P., Baillet, S., Garnero, L., and Martinerie, J. (2003). A theoretical and experimental introduction to the non-invasive study of large-scale neural phase-synchronization in human beings (invited paper). International Journal of Computational Cognition 1: 53–77. URL: http://www.YangSky.com/yangijcc.htm
David, O., Garnero, L., Cosmelli, D., and Varela, F. J. (2002). Estimation of neural dynamics from MEG/EEG cortical current density maps: application to the reconstruction of large-scale cortical synchrony. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 49: 975–987.
Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., and Goldsmith, H. H., eds. (2003). Handbook of Affective Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
———. (1986). The Blind Watchmaker. London: Penguin Books.
———. (1989). The Selfish Gene, new ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
———. (1995). River out of Eden: A Darwinian View of Life. New York: Basic Books.
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W. W. Norton.
———. (2003). The hierarchic logic of emergence: untangling the interdependence of evolution and self-organization. In B. Weber and D. Depew, eds., Evolution and Learning: The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered, pp. 273–308 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Decety, J. (in press). A social cognitive neuroscience model of human empathy. In E. Harmon-Jones and P. Winkielman, eds., Fundamentals of Social Neuroscience. New York: Guilford Press.
Decety, J., and Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 527–533.
Deffuant, G., Fuhs, T., Monnert, E., Bourgine, P., and Varela, F. J. (1995). Semi-algebraic networks: an attempt to design geometric autopoietic networks. Artificial Life 2: 157–177.
Dehaene, S., and Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79: 1–37.
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Two approaches to mental images. In D. C. Dennett, Brainstorms: Philosophical Essays on Mind and Psychology, pp. 174–189. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (1982). How to study consciousness empirically, or nothing comes to mind. Synthese 59: 159–180.
———. (1991a). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown.
———. (1991b). Real patterns. Journal of Philosophy 88: 27–51.
———. (1995a). Darwin’s Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
———. (1995b). The unimagined preposterousness of zombies. Journal of Consciousness Studies 2: 322–326.
———. (1998). No bridge over the stream of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 753–756.
———. (2002a). Does your brain use the images in it, and if so, how? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 189–190.
———. (2002b). How could I be wrong? How wrong could I be? Journal of Consciousness Studies 9: 13–16.
———. (2003). Who’s on first? Heterophenomenology explained. In A. Jack and A. Roepstorff, eds., Trusting the Subject? The Use of Introspective Evidence in Cognitive Science. Volume 1, pp. 19–30. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
———. (2005). Sweet Dreams: Philosophical Obstacles to a Science of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Dennett, D. C., and Kinsbourne, M. (1992). Time and the observer: the where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15: 183–247.
Depraz, N. (1995). Transcendence et incarnation: le statut de l’intersubjectivité comme altérité à soi chez Husserl. Paris: Librarie Philosophique J. Vrin.
———. (1997). La traduction de Leib, une crux phaenomenologica. Etudes Phénoménologiques 3.
———. (1999a). Review of Ichiro Yamaguchi, Ki als leibhaftige Vernunft. Alter 7: 63–68.
———. (1999b). The phenomenological reduction as praxis. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 95–110.
———. (1999c). When transcendental genesis encounters the naturalization project. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 464–489. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
———. (2001a). Lucidité du corps. De l’empiricisme transcendental en phénoménologie. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (2001b). The Husserlian theory of intersubjectivity as alterology: emergent theories and wisdom traditions in the light of genetic phenomenology. In E. Thompson, ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, pp. 169–178. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (2001): 169–178.
Depraz, N., Varela, F. J., and Vermersch, P. (2000). The gesture of awareness: an account of its structural dynamics. In M. Velmans, ed., Investigating Phenomenal Consciousness, pp. 121–136. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Press.
———. (2003). On Becoming Aware: A Pragmatics of Experiencing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Press.
De Preester, H. (2003). Meaning: what’s the matter? Intrinsic relations and the organism. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum 7: 195–205.
DeRobertis, E. M., and Sasai, Y. (1996). A common plan for dorsoventral patterning in bilateria. Nature 380: 37–40.
Derryberry, D., and Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. Niedenthal and S. Kitayama, eds., The Heart’s Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention, pp. 167–196. New York: Academic Press.
Descartes, R. (1986). Meditations on First Philosophy, with Selections from the Objections and Replies, trans, J. Cottingham. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
———. (2002). On the possibility of animal empathy. In T. Manstead, N. Fridja, and A. Fischer, eds., Feelings and Emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium, pp. 381–401. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Di Paolo, E. A. (2005). Autopoiesis, adaptivity, teleology, agency. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4: 429–452.
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
———. (2001). A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York: W. W. Norton.
Doolittle, W. F. (1981). Is nature really motherly? CoEvolution Quarterly 29: 58–63.
———. (1987). From selfish DNA to Gaia: one molecular biologist’s view of the evolutionary process. In J. M. Robson, ed., Origin and Evolution of the Universe: Evidence for Design?, pp. 59–76. Kingston, Ontario: McGillQueen’s University Press.
Dretske, F. (1995a). Naturalizing the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (1995b). Mental events as structuring causes of behavior. In J. Heil and A. Mele, eds., Mental Causation, pp. 121–136. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Dreyfus, G., and Thompson, E. (2007). Asian perspectives: Indian theories of mind. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson, eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dreyfus, H. (1972). What Computers Can’t Do. New York: Harper and Row.
———. (1982). Introduction. In H. Dreyfus and H. Harrison, eds., Husserl, Intentionality and Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1988). Husserl’s epiphenomenology. In H. R. Otto and J. A. Tuedio, eds., Perspectives on Mind, pp. 85–104. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
———. (1991). Being-In-The-World: A Commentary on Heidegger’s Being and Time, Division I. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1992). What Computers Still Can’t Do. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (2000). Responses. In M. Wrathall and J. Malpas, eds., Heidegger, Coping, and Cognitive Science. Essays in Honor of Hubert L. Dreyfus, vol. 2, pp. 311–349. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (2002). Intelligence without representation—Merleau-Ponty’s critique of mental representation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 367–383.
———. (2005). Merleau-Ponty and recent cognitive science. In T. Carman and M. B. N. Hansen, eds., The Cambridge Companion to Merleau-Ponty, pp. 129–150. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dreyfus, H., and Dreyfus, S. (1986). Mind over Machine. New York: Free Press.
Drummond, J. J. (2003). The structure of intentionality. In D. Welton, ed., The New Husserl: A Critical Reader, pp. 65–92. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Dupré, J. (1993). The Disorder of Things. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Dupuy, J.-P. (2000). The Mechanization of the Mind: On the Origins of Cognitive Science. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Dyson, F. (1985). Origins of Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Empathy and sympathy. In M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones, eds., Handbook of Emotions, 2d ed., pp. 677–691. New York: Guilford Press.
Eisenberg, N., and Strayer, J. (1987). Empathy and Its Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ekman, P., and Davidson, R. J., eds. (1994). The Nature of Emotion. Fundamental Questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Elman, J. (1991). Representation and structure in connectionist models. In G. Altman, ed., Cognitive Models of Speech Processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Emmeche, C., Køppe, S., and Stjernfelt, F. (2000). Levels, emergence, and three versions of downward causation. In P. B. Andersen, C. Emmeche, N. O. Finnemann, and P. V. Christiansen, eds., Downward Causation: Minds, Bodies and Matter, pp. 13–34. Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University Press.
Engel, A. K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 704–716.
Engel, A. K., and Singer, W. (2001). Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 16–25.
Epstein, M. (1996). Thoughts without a Thinker: Psychotherapy from a Buddhist Perspective. New York: Basic Books.
Faure, P., and Korn, H. (2001). Is there chaos in the brain? I. Concepts of nonlinear dynamics and methods of investigation. Comptes Rendus de l’Académie des Sciences 324: 773–793.
Fleischaker, G. R. (1988). Autopoiesis: the status of its system logic. Biosystems 22: 37–49.
———. (1990a). Origins of life: an operational definition. Origins of Life and Evolution of the Biosphere 20: 127–137.
———. (1990b). Symbols and dynamics: two different system ideas. Perceiving-Acting Workshop Review [PAW Review], Center for the Study of Perception and Action [CEPSPA] 5: 30–32.
———. (1994). A few precautionary words concerning terminology. In G. R. Fleischaker, S. Colonna, and P. L. Luisi, eds., Self-Production of Supramolecular Structures. From Synthetic Structures to Models of Minimal Living Systems, pp. 33–41. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Fodor, J. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
———. (1981). Representations. Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Fontana, W., and Buss, L. W. (1994). What would be conserved if “the tape were played twice”? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 91: 757–761.
Fontana, W., Wagner, G., and Buss, L. W. (1994). Beyond digital naturalism. Artificial Life 1: 211–227.
Forman, R. K. C., ed. (1990). The Problem of Pure Consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.
Franklin, S. (1995). Artificial Minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Freeman, W. J. (1995). Societies of Brains: A Study in the Neuroscience of Love and Hate. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
———. (1999a). How Brains Make Up Their Minds. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.
———. (1999b). Consciousness, intentionality, and causality. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 143–172.
———. (2000). Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In M. D. Lewis and I. Granic, eds., Emotion, Development, and Self-Organization: Dynamic Systems Approaches to Emotional Development, pp. 209–235. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
———. (2003). The wave packet: an action potential for the 21st century. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience 2: 3–30.
Freud, S. (1915). The unconscious (1915). In S. Freud, On Metapsychology: The Theory of Psychoanalysis, pp. 159–222. The Pelican-Freud Library, vol. 11. London: Penguin Books, 1984.
———. (1923). The Ego and the Id (1923). In S. Freud, On Metapsychology: The Theory of Psychoanalysis, pp. 339–407. The Pelican-Freud Library, vol. 11. London: Penguin Books, 1984.
Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291: 1560–1563.
Friston, K. J. (2000a). The labile brain. I. Neuronal transients and nonlinear coupling. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B. 355: 215–236.
———. (2000b). The labile brain. II. Transients, complexity, and selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B. 355: 237–252.
Gallagher, S. (1986a). Body image and body schema: a conceptual clarification. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 7: 541–554.
———. (1986b). Lived body and environment. Research in Phenomenology 16: 139–170.
———. (1995). Body schema and intentionality. In J. L. Bermúdez, A. Marcel, and N. Eilan, eds., The Body and the Self, pp. 225–244. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1997). Mutual enlightenment: recent phenomenology in cognitive science. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4: 195–214.
———. (1998). The Inordinance of Time. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (2003). Bodily self-awareness and object perception. Theoria et Historia Scientarium: International Journal for Interdisciplinary Studies 7: 53–68.
Gallagher, S., and Varela, F. J. (2003). Redrawing the map and resetting the time: phenomenology and the cognitive sciences. In E. Thompson, ed., The Problem of Consciousness: New Essays in Phenomenological Philosophy of Mind. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, supplementary vol. 29, pp. 93–132. Calgary: University of Alberta Press.
Gallese, V. (2001). The “shared manifold” hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy. In E. Thompson, ed., Between Ourselves: Second Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, pp. 33–50. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (2001): 33–50.
Gallese, V., Keysers, C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 396–403.
Gallup, G., Jr. (1998). Can animals empathize? Yes. Scientific American 9: 65–75.
Gardner, H. (1985). The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Basic Books.
Garfinkel, A. (1981). Forms of Explanation: Rethinking the Questions in Social Theory. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Garrett, B. (in press). What the history of vitalism teaches us about consciousness and the “hard problem.” Philosophy and Phenomenological Research.
Gatlin, L. (1972). Information and the Living System. New York: Columbia University Press.
Gendlin, E. T. (1981). Focusing. New York: Bantam.
———. (1997). Experiencing and the Creation of Meaning: A Philosophical and Psychological Approach to the Subjective. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Gerhart, J., and Kirschner, M. (1997). Cells, Embryos, and Evolution: Toward a Cellular and Developmental Understanding of Phenotypic Variation and Evolutionary Adaptability. Malden, MA: Blackwell Science Publications.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gilbert, S. F., Opitz, J. M., and Raff, R. A. (1996). Resynthesizing evolutionary and developmental biology. Developmental Biology 173: 357–372.
Godfrey-Smith, P. (1996). Spencer and Dewey on life and mind. In M. Boden, ed., The Philosophy of Artificial Life, pp. 314–331. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
———. (2000a). Information, arbitrariness, and selection: comments on Maynard Smith. Philosophy of Science 67: 202–207.
———. (2000b). On the theoretical role of “genetic coding.” Philosophy of Science 67: 26–44.
Gold, I. (2002). Interpreting the neuroscience of imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 190–191.
Goldman, A. I. (1997). Science, publicity, and consciousness. Philosophy of Science 64: 525–545.
———. (2000). Can science know when you’re conscious? Epistemological foundations of consciousness research. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7: 3–22.
———. (2004). Epistemology and the evidential status of introspective reports. In A. Jack and A. Roepstorff, eds., Trusting the Subject? The Use of Introspective Evidence in Cognitive Science. vol. 2. pp. 1–16. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Goldstein, K. (1995). The Organism: A Holistic Approach to Biology Derived from Pathological Data in Man. New York: Zone Books.
Goodwin, B. (1994). How The Leopard Changed Its Spots: The Evolution of Complexity. New York: Scribner’s.
Gould, S. J. (1989). Wonderful Life: The Burgess Shale and the Nature of History. New York: W. W. Norton.
Gould, S. J., and Lewontin, R. C. (1978). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 205: 581–598. Page references in the text are to the version reprinted in E. Sober, ed., Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Biology, pp. 252–270. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book, 1984.
Gray, R. D. (1992). Death of the gene: developmental systems strike back. In P. E. Griffiths, ed., Trees of Life: Essays in Philosophy of Biology, pp. 165–209. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Grice, H. P. (2002). Some remarks about the senses. In A. Noë and E. Thompson, eds., Vision and Mind: Readings in the Philosophy of Perception, pp. 35–54, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Griffiths, P. E. (1997). What Emotions Really Are: The Problem of Psychological Categories. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Griffiths, P. E., and Gray, R. D. (1994). Developmental systems and evolutionary explanation. Journal of Philosophy 91: 277–304.
Griffiths, P. E., and Knight, R. D. (1998). What is the developmentalist challenge? Philosophy of Science 65: 253–258.
Griffiths, P. E., and Stotz, K. (2000). How the mind grows: a developmental perspective on the biology of cognition. Synthese 122: 29–51.
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 377–396.
Gupta, B. (1998). The Disinterested Witness: A Fragment of Advaita Vedanta Phenomenology. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (2003). Cit. Consciousness. New Delhi: Oxford University Press.
———. (2004). Advaita Vedanta and Husserl’s Phenomenology. Husserl Studies 20: 119–134.
Gurwitsch, A. (1964). The Field of Consciousness. Pittsburgh, PA: Dusquesne University Press.
Güzeldere, G. (1997). The many faces of consciousness: a field guide. In N. Block, O. Flanagan, and G. Güzeldere, eds., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates, pp. 1–67. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Haken, H. (1983). Synergtics: An Introduction. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., and Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics 51: 347–356.
Hall, B. K., and Olson, W. M. (2003). Keywords and Concepts in Evolutionary Developmental Biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hanna, R., and Thompson, E. (2003). The mind-body-body problem. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum: International Journal for Interdisciplinary Studies 7: 24–44.
Harman, G. (1997). The intrinsic quality of experience. In N. Block, O. Flanagan, and G. Güzeldere, eds., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates, pp. 663–676. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Hattiangadi, J. (2005). The emergence of minds in space and time. In D. M. Johnson and C. Ernelling, eds., The Mind as a Scientific Object: Between Brain and Culture, pp. 79–100. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hawthorne, J., and Silberstein, M. (1995). For whom the Bell arguments toll. Synthese 102: 99–138.
Healey, R. A. (1991). Holism and nonseparability. Journal of Philosophy 88: 393–421.
Hebb, D. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: John Wiley.
Heidegger, M. (1982). The Basic Problems of Phenomenology, trans. A. Hofstadter. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
———. (1985). History of the Concept of Time, trans. T. Kisiel. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
———. (1995). The Fundamental Concepts of Metaphysics: World, Finitude, Solitude, trans. W. McNeill and N. Walker. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
———. (1996). Being and Time, trans. J. Stambaugh. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Held, K. (2003). Husserl’s phenomenological method. In D. Welton, ed., The New Husserl: A Critical Reader, pp. 3–31. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Hendriks-Jansen, H. (1995). Catching Ourselves in the Act: Situated Activity, Interactive Emergence, Evolution, and Human Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Hering, E. (1964). Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense, trans. L. M. Hurvich and D. Jameson. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Ho, M.-W., and Saunders, P. (1984). Beyond Neo-Darwinism. New York: Academic Press.
Holland, P. W. H. (1999). The future of evolutionary developmental biology. Nature 402: Supplement: C41–C44.
Hopkins, R. (1998). Picture, Image, and Experience. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Humphrey, N. (2000). How to solve the mind-body problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4: 5–20.
———. (2001). Seeing it my way: sensation, perception—and feeling red. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 997.
Humphreys, P. (1996). Aspects of emergence. Philosophical Topics 24: 53–70.
———. (1997a). Emergence, not supervenience. Philosophy of Science 64 (Proceedings): S337–S345.
———. (1997b). How properties emerge. Philosophy of Science 64: 1–17.
Hurley, S. L. (1998). Consciousness in Action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hurley, S. L., and Noë, A. (2003). Neural plasticity and consciousness. Biology and Philosophy 18: 131–168.
Husserl, E. (1960). Cartesian Meditations: An Introduction to Phenomenology, trans. Dorian Cairns. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (1970). The Crisis of European Sciences and Transcendental Phenomenology, trans. D. Carr. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (1973). Zur Phänomenologie der Intersubjektivität, Dreiter Teil: 1929–1935. Husserliana, vol. 15. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
———. (1975). Experience and Judgment, trans. J. S. Churchill. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (1977). Philosophical Psychology, trans. J. Scanlon. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
———. (1980). Phenomenology and the Foundations of the Sciences: Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomnology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy. Third Book, trans. T. E. Klein and W. E. Pohl. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
———. (1983). Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy, First Book, trans. F. Kersten. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
———. (1989). Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy, Second Book, trans. R. Rojcewicz and A. Schuwer. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (1991). On the Phenomenology of the Consciousness of Internal Time (1893–1917), trans. J. B. Brough. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (1997). Thing and Space: Lectures of 1907, trans. R. Rojcewicz. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (2000). Logical Investigations, vols. 1 and 2, trans. J Findlay. New York: Humanity Books/Prometheus Books. English translation originally published in 1970.
———. (2001). Analyses Concerning Passive and Active Synthesis: Lectures on Transcendental Logic, trans. A. J. Steinbock. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (2006). Phantasy, Image Consciousness, and Memory (1898–1925), trans. J. B. Brough. Berlin: Springer.
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Hutchinson, W., Davis, K., Lozano, A., Tasker, R., and Dostrovsky, J. (1999). Pain related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience 2: 403–405.
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., and Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286: 2526–2528.
Isles, A. R., and Wilkinson, L. S. (2000). Imprinted genes, cognition and behavior. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 309–318.
Jablonka, E. (2001). The systems of inheritance. In S. Oyama, P. E. Griffiths, and R. D. Gray, eds., Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and Evolution, pp. 99–116. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jablonka, E., and Szathmáry, E. (1995). The evolution of information storage and heredity. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 10: 206–211.
Jack, A. I., and Roepstorff, A. (2002). Introspection and cognitive brain mapping: from stimulus-response to script-report. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 333–339.
———, eds. (2003). Trusting the Subject? Volume 1. The Use of Introspective Evidence in Cognitive Science. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
———. (2004). Trusting the Subject? vol. 2. The Use of Introspective Evidence in Cognitive Science. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
James, W. (1981). The Principles of Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
———. (1985). Psychology: The Briefer Course. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
Jantsch, E. (1980). The Self-Organizing Universe: Scientific and Human Implications of the Emerging Paradigm of Evolution. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Johnson, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Imagination, Reason, and Meaning. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
———. (1993). Moral Imagination: Implications of Cognitive Science for Ethics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Jonas, H. (1966). The Phenomenon of Life: Toward a Philosophical Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Reprinted by Northwestern University Press, 2000.
———. (1968). Biological foundations of individuality. International Philosophical Quarterly 8: 231–251.
———. (1996). Mortality and Morality: A Search for the Good after Auschwitz. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in Action: Intentional Behavior as a Complex System. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Juarrero-Roqué, A. (1985). Self-organization: Kant’s conception of teleology and modern chemistry. Review of Metaphysics 39: 107–135.
Kabat-Zinn J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. New York: Dell.
Kahana, M. J., Seelig, D., and Madsen, J. R. (2001). Theta returns. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11: 739–744.
Kant, I. (1987). Critique of Judgment, trans. W. S. Pluhar. Indianapolis, IN: Hacket Publishing Company.
Kauffman, S. A. (1991). Antichaos and adaptation. Scientific American 265: 78–84.
———. (1993). Origins of Order: Self-Organization and Selection in Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
———. (1995). At Home in the Universe. New York: Oxford University Press.
———. (2000). Investigations. New York: Oxford University Press.
Keller, E. F. (1984). A Feeling for the Organism: The Life and Work of Barbara McClintock. New York: W. H. Freeman.
———. (1995). Refiguring Life: Metaphors of Twentieth Century Biology. New York: Columbia University Press.
———. (2000). The Century of the Gene. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kellert, S. H. (1993). In the Wake of Chaos. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Kelso, J. A. S., Fuchs, A., Lancaster, R., Holroyd, T., Cheyne, D., and Weinberg, H. (1998). Dynamic cortical activity in the human brain reveals motor equivalence. Nature 392: 814–818.
Kelso, J. A. S., and Kay, B. A. (1987). Information and control: a macroscopic analysis of perception-action coupling. In H. Heuer and A. F. Sanders, eds., Perspectives on Perception and Action, pp. 3–32. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kim, J. (1993). Supervenience and Mind: Selected Philosophical Essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
———. (1998). Mind in a Physical World: An Essay on the Mind-Body Problem and Mental Causation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (1999). Making sense of emergence. Philosophical Studies 95: 3–36.
Kind, A. (2003). What’s so transparent about transparency? Philosophical Studies 115: 225–244.
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liveright.
Korn, H., and Faure, P. (2003). Is there chaos in the brain? II. Experimental evidence and related models. Comptes Rendus Biologie 326: 787–840.
Koshland, D. E., Jr. (1977). A response regulator model in a simple sensory system. Science 196: 1055–1063.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
———. (1994). Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L. (2003). Mental imagery: against the nihilistic hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 109–111.
Kosslyn, S. M., Pinker, S., Smith, G. E., and Schwartz, S. P. (1981). On the demystification of mental imagery. In N. Block, ed., Imagery, pp. 131–150. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Kosslyn, S. M., and Pomerantz, J. R. (1981). Imagery, propositions, and the form of internal representations. In N. Block, ed., Readings in the Philosophy of Psychology, vol. 2, pp. 150–169. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G. (2006). The Case for Mental Imagery. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kriegel, U. (2003a). Consciousness as intransitive self-consciousness: two views and an argument. Canadian Journal of Philosophy 33: 103–132.
———. (2003b). Consciousness as sensory quality and as implicit selfawareness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 2: 1–26.
———. (2004). The functional role of consciousness: a phenomenological approach. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 3: 171–193.
———. (2005). Naturalizing subjective character. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 71: 23–57.
Kronz, F. M. (1998). Nonseparability and quantum chaos. Philosophy of Science 65: 50–75.
Kronz, F. M., and Tiehen, J. T. (2002). Emergence and quantum mechanics. Philosophy of Science 69: 324–347.
Kuhn, T. (1970). The Stucture of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J., and Varela, F. J. (1999). Measuring phase-synchrony in brain signals. Human Brain Mapping 8: 194–208.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lambert, D. M., and Hughes, A. J. (1988). Keywords and concepts in structuralist and functionalist biology. Journal of Theoretical Biology 133: 133–145.
Lambie, J. A., and Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychological Review 109: 219–259.
Lane, R. D., and Nadel, L., eds. (2000). Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Langsdorf, L. (1985). Review of Dreyfus 1982. Husserl Studies 3: 303–311.
Langton, C. G., ed. (1989). Artificial Life. Santa Fe Studies in the Sciences of Complexity, vol. 6. Redwood City, CA: Addison-Welsey.
Latour, B. (1993). We Have Never Been Modern, trans. C. Porter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Laycock, S. W. (1994). Mind as Mirror and the Mirroring of Mind. Buddhist Reflections on Western Phenomenology. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Leask, I. (2002). Edith Stein and others. Journal of the British Society for Phenomenology 33: 286–298.
LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon Schuster.
———. (2002). Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. London: Penguin Books.
Legrand, D. (2003). How not to find the neural signature of self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 12: 544–546.
———. (2005). Transparently oneself. Psyche 11 (5), June 2005. URL: http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/symposia/metzinger/LEGRAND.pdf
———. (2006). The bodily self: the sensorimotor roots of pre-reflexive self-consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 5: 89–118.
Lennox, J. G. (1992). Teleology. In E. F. Keller and E. Lloyd, eds., Keywords in Evolutionary Biology, pp. 324–333. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Letelier, J. C., Marín, G., and Mpodozis, J. (2003). Autopoietic and (M,R) systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology 222: 261–272.
Le Van Quyen, M. (2003). Disentangling the dynamic core: a research program for neurodynamics at the large-scale. Biological Research 36: 67–88.
Le Van Quyen, M., Adam, C., Lachaux, J.-P., Martinerie, J., Baulac, M., Renault, B., and Varela, F. J. (1997). Temporal patterns in human epileptic activity are modulated by perceptual discriminations. Neuroreport 8: 1703–1710.
Le Van Quyen, M., Martinerie, J., Adam, C., and Varela, F. J. (1997). Unstable periodic orbits in human epileptic activity. Physical Review E 56: 3401–3411.
Le Van Quyen, M., and Petitmengin, C. (2002). Neuronal dynamics and conscious experience: an example of reciprocal causation before epileptic seizures. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 169–180.
Le Van Quyen, M., Schuster, H., and Varela, F. J. (1996). Fast rhythms can emerge from slow neuronal oscillators. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos 6: 1807–1816.
Levenson, R., and Reuf, A. M. (1992). Empathy: a physiological substrate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63: 234–246.
Levine, J. (1983). Materialism and qualia: the explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 64: 354–361.
Levins, R., and Lewontin, R. (1985). The Dialectical Biologist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Levinson, J. (1998). Wolheim on pictorial representation. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 56: 227–233.
Lewis, M. D. (2000). Emotional self-organization at three time scales. In M. D. Lewis and I. Granic, eds., Emotion, Development, and Self-Organization: Dynamic Systems Approaches to Emotional Development, pp. 37–69. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
———. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 169–194.
Lewis, M. D., and Granic, I., eds. (2000). Emotion, Development, and Self-Organization: Dynamic Systems Approaches to Emotional Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lewis, M. L., and Haviland-Jones, J. M., eds. (2000). Handbook of Emotions, 2d ed. New York: Guilford Press.
Lewontin, R. (1978). Adaptation. Scientific American 239: 212–230.
———. (1993). Biology as Ideology: The Doctrine of DNA. New York: Harper-Collins.
Llinas, R., Ribary, U., Contreras, D., and Pedroarena, C. (1998). The neuronal basis for consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B 353: 1801–1818.
Lloyd, D. (2002). Functional MRI and the study of human consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14: 818–831.
———. (2003). Radiant Cool: A Novel Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Lopes, D. (1996). Understanding Pictures. Clarendon: Oxford University Press.
Lovelock, J. E. (1979). Gaia: A New Look at Life on Earth. New York: Oxford University Press.
———. (1987). The ecopoiesis of daisy world. In J. M. Robson, ed., Origin and Evolution of the Universe: Evidence for Design? pp. 153–166. Kingston, Ontariol: McGill-Queen’s University Press.
———. (1988). The Ages of Gaia. New York: W. W. Norton.
———. (1991). Gaia: a planetary emergent phenomenon. In W. I. Thompson, ed., Gaia 2: Emergence. The New Science of Becoming, pp. 30–49. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
Lovelock, J. E., and Margulis, L. (1974). Homeostatic tendencies of the earth’s atmosphere. Origins of Life 5: 93–103.
Luisi, P. L. (1993). Defining the transition to life: self-replicating bounded structures and chemical autopoiesis. In W. D. Stein and F. J. Varela, eds., Thinking about Biology. Santa Fe Studies in the Sciences of Complexity, Lecture Notes, vol. 3, pp. 17–39. NJ: Addison-Wesley.
———. (1998). About various definitions of life. Origins of Life and Evolution of the Biosphere 28: 613–622.
———. (2003). Autopoiesis: a review and a reappraisal. Naturwissenschaften 90: 49–59.
Luisi, P. L., Lazcano, A., and Varela, F. J. (1996). What is life? Defining life and the transition to life. In M. Rizzotti, ed., Defining Life: The Central Problem in Theoretical Biology, pp. 146–167. Padua: Edizione Università di Padova.
Luisi, P. L., and Varela, F. J. (1989). Self-replicating micelles—a chemical version of a minimal autopoietic system. Origins of Life and Evolution of the Biosphere 19: 663–643.
Lusthaus, D. (2002). Buddhist Phenomenology: A Philosophical Investigation of Yogacara Buddhism and the Ch’eng Wei-shih tun. London: Routledge/Curzon Press.
Lutz, A. (2002). Toward a neurophenomenology as an account of generative passages: a first empirical case study. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 133–167.
Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: an introduction. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson, eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lutz, A., Lachaux, J.-P., Martinerie, J., and Varela, F. J. (2002). Guiding the study of brain dynamics by using first-person data: synchrony patterns correlate with ongoing conscious states during a simple visual task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99: 1586–1591.
Lutz, A., and Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology: integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10: 31–52.
Mach, E. (1959). The Analysis of Sensations and the Relation of the Physical to the Psychical, trans. C. A. Williams. New York: Dover Publications.
Mack, A., and Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Maes, P., ed. (1990). Designing Autonomous Agents. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Maier, S. F., and Watkins, L. R. (1998). Cytokines for psychologists: implications of bi-directional immune-to-brain communication for understanding behavior, mood, and cognition. Psychological Review 105: 83–107.
Marbach, E. (1993). Mental Representation and Consciousness: Towards a Phenomenological Theory of Representation and Reference. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Margulis, L. (1984). Early Life. Boston: Jones and Bartlett.
———. (1991). Biologists can’t define life. In C. Barlow, ed., From Gaia to Selfish Genes: Selected Writings in the Life Sciences, pp. 236–238. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1993). Symbiosis in Cell Evolution, 2d ed. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
———. (1998). Symbiotic Planet: A New Look at Evolution. New York: Basic Books.
———. (2001). The conscious cell. In P. C. Marijúan, ed., Cajal and Consciousness: Scientific Approaches to Consciousness on the Centennial of Ramon y Cajal’s Textura, pp. 55–70. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 929. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Margulis, L., and Lovelock, J. E. (1974). Biological modulation of the Earth’s atmosphere. Icarus 21: 471–489.
Margulis, L., and Sagan, D. (1986). Microcosmos: Four Billion Years of Microbial Evolution. New York: Summit Books.
———. (1995). What Is Life? New York: Simon & Schuster.
———. (2002). Acquiring Genomes. A Theory of the Origins of Species. New York: Basic Books.
Margulis, L., and Schwartz, K. V. (1988). Five Kingdoms: An Illustrated Guide to the Phyla of Life on Earth, 2d ed. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Martin, M. G. F. (2002). The transparency of experience. Mind and Language 17: 376–425.
Matthews, G. B. (1991). Consciousness and life. In D. M. Rosenthal, ed., The Nature of Mind, pp. 63–70. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Maturana, H. R. (1969). The neurophysiology of cognition. In P. Garvin, ed., Cognition: A Multiple View, pp. 3–23. New York: Spartan Books.
———. (1970). Biology of cognition. In H. R. Maturana and F. J. Varela, Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living, pp. 2–58. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 43. Dordrecht: D. Reidel, 1980.
———. (1975). The organization of the living: a theory of the living organization. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 7: 313–332.
———. (1980a). Autopoiesis: reproduction, heredity and evolution. In M. Zeleny, ed., Autopoiesis, Dissipative Structures, and Spontaneous Social Orders, pp. 45–79. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
———. (1980b). Man and society. In F. Benseler, P. M. Hejl, and W. K. Köck, eds., Autopoiesis, Communication, and Society. New York: Campus Verlag.
Maturana, H. R., and Varela, F. J. (1973). De máquinas y seres vivos: Una teoría de la organizacíon biológica. Santiago, Chile: Editorial Universitaria.
———. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 42. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
———. (1987). The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding. Boston: Shambala Press/New Science Library.
Maudlin, T. (1998). Part and whole in quantum mechanics. In E. Castellani, ed., Interpreting Bodies: Classical and Quantum Objects in Modern Physics, pp. 46–60. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Maynard Smith, J. (1986). The Problems of Biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
———. (1993). The Theory of Evolution, 2d ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
———. (1998). Shaping Life: Genes, Embryos and Evolution. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.
———. (2000a). The concept of information in biology. Philosophy of Science 67: 177–194.
———. (2000b). Reply to commentaries. Philosophy of Science 67: 214–218.
Maynard Smith, J., Burian, R., Kauffman, S., Alberch, P., Campbell, J., Goodwin, B., Lande, R., Raup, D., and Wolpert, L. (1985). Developmental constraints and evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology 60: 265–287.
McClelland, J., Rummelhart, D., and the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, vols. 1 and 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McCulloch, G. (1994). Using Sartre: An Analytical Introduction to Early Sartrean Themes. London: Routledge Press.
McGinn, C. (1991). The Problem of Consciousness. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
———. (2004). Mindsight: Image, Dream, Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
McMullin, B. (1997a). Computational autopoiesis: the original algorithm. Working Paper 97–01–001, Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501, USA. Available at: http://www.eeng.dcu.ie/˜alife/bmcm-ecal97/
———. (1997b). Modelling autopoiesis: harder than it may seem! Presented at Biology, Cognition, Language & Society: An International Symposium on Autopoiesis, Belo Horizonte, Brazil, November 1997. Available at: http://www.eeng.dcu.ie/˜mcmullin/
———. (1999). Some remarks on autocatalysis and autopoiesis. Presented at the workshop Closure: Emergent Organizations and Their Dynamics, May 3–5, 1999. University of Ghent, Belgium. Available at: http://www.eeng.dcu.ie/˜mcmullin/
———. (2001). An intriguing journey: a review of Investigations by Stuart Kauffman. Complexity 6: 22–23. Available at: http://www.eeng.dcu.ie/˜mcmullin/
McMullin, B., and Varela, F. J. (1997). Rediscovering computational autopoeisis. In P. Husbands and I. Harvey, eds., Fourth European Confrence on Artificial Life, pp. 38–48. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book. Available at: http://www.eeng.dcu.ie/˜mcmullin
Melloni, L., and Rodriguez, E. (2005). Phase synchronization but not gamma oscillations correlates with conscious perception. Poster Presentation. Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness, 9th Annual Meeting, Pasadena, CA, June 23–27, 2005.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1998). Infant intersubjectivity: broadening the dialogue to include imitation, identity, and intention. In S. Braten, ed., Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, pp. 47–62. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mensch, J. R. (1996). After Modernity: Husserlian Reflections on a Philosophical Tradition. Albany: State University Press of New York.
———. (1998). Instincts—a Husserlian account. Husserl Studies 14: 219–237.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of Perception, trans. Colin Smith. London: Routledge Press.
———. (1963). The Structure of Behavior, trans. A. Fisher. Pittsburgh, PA: Dusquene University Press.
———. (1964). Signs, trans. Richard C. McCleary. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (1968). The Visible and the Invisible, trans. A. Lingis. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (2001). The Incarnate Subject: Malebranche, Biran, and Bergson on the Union of Body and Soul, trans. P. B. Milan. Amherst, NY: Humanity Books.
———. (2003). Nature: Course Notes from the Collège de France. Compiled with notes by D. Sé gard, trans. R. Vallier. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Metzinger, T., ed. (2000). Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (2003). Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Mikulecky, D. C. (2000). Robert Rosen: the well-posed question and its answer—why are organisms different from machines? System Research and Behavioral Science 17: 419–432.
Mingers, J. (1995). Self-Producing Systems: The Implications and Applications of Autopoieisis. New York: Plenum Press.
Mohanty, J. N. (1989). Transcendental Phenomenology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Monod, J. (1971). Chance and Necessity, trans. A. Wainhouse. New York: Vintage Books.
Montero, B. (1999). The body problem. Nous 33: 183–200.
———. (2001). Post-physicalism. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 61–80.
Moody, T. (1994). Conversations with zombies. Journal of Consciousness Studies 1: 196–200.
Moore, G. E. (1993). The refutation of idealism. In G. E. Moore, Selected Writings, pp. 23–44. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Moran, D. (2000). Introduction to Phenomenology. London: Routledge Press.
Moreno, A., and Barandiaran, X. (2004). A naturalized account of the inside-outside dichotomy. Philosophica 73: 11–26.
Morowitz, H. J. (1992). Beginnings of Cellular Life: Metabolism Recapitulates Biogenesis. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
———. (2001). The epistemic paradox of mind and matter. In P. C. Marijúan, ed., Cajal and Consciousness: Scientific Approaches to Consciousness on the Centennial of Ramon y Cajal’s Textura, pp. 50–54. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 929. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Morowitz, H. J., Heinz, B., and Deamer, D. W. (1988). The chemical logic of a minimum protocell. Origins of Life and Evolution of the Biosphere 18 (1988): 281–287. Reprinted in D. W. Deamer and G. R. Fleischaker, eds., Origins of Life: The Central Concepts, pp. 263–269. Boston: Jones and Bartlett, 1994.
Moss, L. (1992). A kernel of truth? On the reality of the genetic program. In D. Hull, M. Forbes, and K. Okruhlik, eds., Proceedings of the Philosophy of Science Association [PSA] 1: 335–348.
———. (2003). What Genes Can’t Do. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Moustakas, C. (1994). Phenomenological Research Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Murphy, M. P., and O’Neil, L. A. J., eds. (1995). What Is Life? The Next Fifty Years: Speculations on the Future of Biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Myin, E., and O’Regan, J. K. (2002). Perceptual consciousness, access to modality, and skill theories: a way to naturalize phenomenology? Journal of Consciousness Studies 9: 27–46.
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 83: 435–450. (Quoted passages in the text are from the reprinted version in T. Nagel, Mortal Questions, pp. 165–180. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1979.)
———. (1979). Mortal Questions. New York: Cambridge University Press.
———. (1980). Armstrong on the mind. In N. Block, ed., Reading in the Philosophy of Psychology, vol. 1, pp. 200–206. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Originally published in Philosophical Review 79 (1970): 394–403.
Newell, A., and Simon, H. (1976). Computer science as empirical inquiry: symbols and search. Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery 19: 113–126. Reprinted in J. Haugeland, ed., Mind Design II, pp. 81–110. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book, 1997.
Noë, A. (2002a). Is perspectival self-consciousness non-conceptual? Philosophical Quarterly 52: 185–195.
———. (2002b). Is the visual world a grand illusion? Journal of Consciousness Studies 9: 1–12.
———, ed. (2002c). Is the Visual World a Grand Illusion? Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
———. (2004). Action in Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Noë, A., Pessoa, L., and Thompson, E. (2000). Beyond the grand illusion: what change blindness really teaches us about vision. Visual Cognition 7: 93–106.
Noë, A., and Thompson, E. (2004a). Are there neural correlates of consciousness? Journal of Consciousness Studies 11: 3–28.
———. (2004b). Sorting out the neural basis of consciousness: Authors’ reply to commentators. Journal of Consciousness Studies 11: 87–98.
Norton, A. (1995). Dynamics: an introduction. In R. F. Port and T. Van Gelder, eds., Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition, pp. 45–68. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Odling-Smee, F. J. (1988). Niche-constructing phenotypes. In H. C. Plotkin, ed., The Role of Behavior in Evolution, pp. 73–132. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
O’Keefe, J., and Burgess, N. (1999). Theta activity, virtual navigation and the human hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 403–406.
Oparin, A. (1938). The Origin of Life on Earth. London: Macmillan.
O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology 46: 461–488.
O’Regan, J. K., and Noë, A. (2001a). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 939–1011.
———. (2001b). Authors’ response: acting out our sensory experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 1011–1031.
Oyama, S. (1992a). Is phylogeny recapitulating ontogeny. In F. J. Varela and J.-P. Dupuy, eds., Understanding Origins, pp. 227–232. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (1992b). Ontogeny and phylogeny: a case of metarecapitulation? In P. Griffiths, ed., Trees of Life: Essays in Philosophy of Biology, pp. 211–239. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (1993). Constraints and development. Netherlands Journal of Zoology 43: 6–16.
———. (1999). Locating development: locating developmental systems. In K. K. Scholnik, K. Nelson, S. A. Gelman, and P. H. Miller, eds., Conceptual Development: Piaget’s Legacy, pp. 185–208. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
———. (2000a). Evolution’s Eye: A System’s View of the Biology-Culture Divide. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
———. (2000b). The Ontogeny of Information: Developmental Systems and Evolution, 2d ed. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. First edition published by Cambridge University Press, 1985.
———. (2000c). Causal democracy and causal contributions in developmental systems theory. Philosophy of Science 67 (Proceedings): S332–S347.
Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E., and Gray, R. D., eds. (2001). Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pacherie, E. (1999). Leibhaftigkeit and representational theories of perception. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 148–160. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Paley, W. (1996). Natural theology. In M. Ruse, ed., But Is It Science? The Philosophical Question in the Creation/Evolution Controversy, pp. 46–69. New York: Prometheus Books.
Panksepp, J. (1998a). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
———. (1998b). The periconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the evolutionary origins of self. Journal of Consciousness Studies 5: 566–582.
———. (2002). The MacLean legacy and some modern trends in emotion research. In G. A. Cory and R. Gardner, eds., The Evolutionary Neuroethology of Paul MacLean: Convergences and Frontiers, pp. ix–xxviii. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
Parvizi, J., and Damasio, A. (2001). Consciousness and the brainstem. Cognition 79: 135–159.
Patočka, J. 1998. Body, Community, Language, World, trans. E. Kohák. Chicago: Open Court.
Pattee, H. H. (1977). Dynamic and linguistic modes of complex systems. International Journal of General Systems Theory 3: 259–266.
di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research 91: 176–180.
Penfield, W. (1938). The cerebral cortex in man. I. The cerebral cortex and consciousness. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 40: 417–442.
Penfield, W., and Jasper, H. (1954). Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain. Boston: Little, Brown.
Pert, C. B. (1997). Molecules of Emotion. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Pert, C. B., Ruff, M. R., Weber, R. J., and Herkenham, M. (1985). Neuropeptides and their receptors: a psychosomatic network. Journal of Immunology 135: 820s–826s.
Pessoa, L., and de Weerd, P., eds. (2003). Filling-In: From Perceptual Completion to Cortical Reorganization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pessoa, L., Thompson, E., and Noë, A. (1998). Finding out about filling-in: a guide to perceptual completion for visual science and the philosophy of perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 723–802.
Petitmengin, C. (2001). L’expérience intuitive. Paris: L’ Harmattan.
———. (2005). Un exemple de recerche neuro-phénoménologique, l’anticipation des crises d’épilepsie. Intellectica 40: 63–89.
———. (in press). Describing one’s subjective experience in the second person: an interview method for the science of consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.
Petitmengin-Peugeot, C. (1999). The intuitive experience. In F. J. Varela and J. Shear, eds., The View from Within: First-Person Approaches to the Study of Consciousness, pp. 43–78. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Petitot, J. (1992). Physique du sens. Paris: Editions du CNRS.
———. (1994). Phenomenology of perception, qualitative physics, and sheaf mereology. In R. Casati, B. Smith, and G. White, eds., Philosophy and the Cognitive Sciences, pp. 387–408. Hölder-Pichler-Tempsky.
———. (1995). Morphodynamics and attractor syntax: constituency in visual perception and cognitive grammar. In R. F. Port and T. van Gelder, eds., Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition, pp. 227–282. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (1999). Morphological eidetics for a phenomenology of perception. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 330–371. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Petitot, J., and Smith, B. (1996). Physics and the phenomenal world. In R. Poli and P. Simons, eds., Formal Ontology, pp. 233–253. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Petitot, J., Varela, F. J., Pachoud, B., and Roy, J.-M., eds. (1999). Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Poellner, P. (2003). Nonconceptual content, experience, and the self. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10: 32–57.
Pöppel, E. (1988). Mindworks: Time and Consciousness Experience. Boston: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich.
Port, R. F., and van Gelder, T., eds. (1995). Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Povinellli, D. J. (1998). Can animals empathize? Maybe not. Scientific American 9: 65–75.
Povinelli, D. J., and Preuss, T. M. (1995). Theory of mind: evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization. Trends in Neurosciences 18: 418–424.
Preston, S. D., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 1–20.
Price, D., and Barrell, J. (1980). An experiential approach with quantitative methods: a research paradigm. Journal of Humanistic Psychology 20: 75–95.
Price, D., Barrell, J., and Rainville, P. (2002). Integrating experientialphenomenological methods and neuroscience to study neural mechanisms of pain and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 11: 593–608.
Priest, S. (1998). Merleau-Ponty. London: Routledge Press.
Prigogine, I., and Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of Chaos: Man’s New Dialogue with Nature. New York: Bantam.
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9: 129–154.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin 80: 1–24.
———. (1984). Computation and Cognition: Towards a Foundation for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (2002). Mental imagery: in search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 157–238.
———. (2003a). Seeing and Visualizing: It’s Not What You Think. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (2003b). Explaining mental imagery: now you see it, now you don’t. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 111–112.
———. (2003c). Return of the mental image: are there really pictures in the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 113–118.
Quartz, S. (1999). The constructivist brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 48–57.
Quartz, S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: a constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20: 537–596.
Raff, R. A. (1996). The Shape of Life: Genes, Development, and the Evolution of Animal Form. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rainville, P. (2005). Neurophénoménologie des é tats et des contenus de conscience dans l’hypnose et l’analgésie hypnotique. Théologique 12: 15–38.
Ramachandran, V. S., and Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the Brain. New York: William Morrow.
Ramachandran, V. S., and Hirstein, W. (1998). The perception of phantom limbs. Brain 121: 1603–1630.
Rees, G., Krieman, G., and Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3: 261–270.
Rey, G. (1981). Introduction: what are mental images? In N. Block, ed., Readings in Philosophical Psychology, vol. 2, pp. 117–127. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rietveld, E. (2004). The nature of everyday coping: reinterpreting MerleauPonty’s phenomenology of concrete habitual coping in the light of Varela’s naturalization of time-consciousness. Presented at the ASSC-8 (Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness) Conference, June 26–28, 2004, Antwerp, Belgium.
Rodemeyer, L. (2003). Developments in the theory of time-consciousness: an analysis of protention. In D. Welton, ed., The New Husserl: A Critical Reader, pp. 125–154. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Rodriguez, E., George, N., Lachaux, J. P., Martinerie, J., Renault, B., and Varela, F. J. (1999). Perception’s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature 397: 430–433.
Roe, A. W., Pallas, S. L., Kwon, Y. H. and Sur, M. (1990). A map of visual space induced in primary auditory cortex. Science 250: 818–820.
———. (1992). Visual projections routed to the auditory pathway in ferrets. Journal of Neuroscience 12: 3651–3664.
Roepstorff, A. (2001). Brains in scanners: an Umwelt of cognitive neuroscience. Semiotica 134: 747–765.
Rollins, M. (1989). Mental Imagery: On the Limits of Cognitive Science. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Rose, S. (1997). Lifelines: Biology Beyond Determinism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rosen, R. (1991). Life Itself: A Comprehensive Inquiry into the Nature, Origin, and Fabrication of Life. New York: Columbia University Press.
———. (2000). Essays on Life Itself. New York: Columbia University Press.
Rosenthal, D. (1997). A theory of consciousness. In N. Block, O. Flanagan, and G. Güzeldere, eds., The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates, pp. 729–753. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Rowlands, M. (2003). Consciousness: the transcendentalist manifesto. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 2: 205–221.
Roy, J.-M. (1995). Le “Dreyfus Bridge”: Husserlianisme et Fodorisme. Archives de Philosophie 58: 533–548.
———. (2003). Phenomenological claims and the myth of the given. In E. Thompson, ed., The Problem of Consciousness: New Essays in Phenomenological Philosophy of Mind. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, supplementary vol. 29, pp. 1–32. Calgary University of Alberta Press.
Roy, J.-M., Petitot, J., Pachoud, B., and Varela, F. J. (1999). Beyond the gap: an introduction to naturalizing phenomenology. In J. Peitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 1–80. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Rudrauf, D., and Damasio, A. (2005). A conjecture regarding the biological mechanism of subjectivity and feeling. Journal of Consciousness Studies 12: 236–262.
Rudrauf, D., Lutz, A., Cosmelli, D., Lachaux, J.-P., and Le Van Quyen, M. (2003). From autopoiesis to neurophenomenology: Francisco Varela’s exploration of the biophysics of being. Biological Research 36: 27–66.
Ruiz-Mirazo, K., and Moreno, A. (2004). Basic autonomy as a fundamental step in the synthesis of life. Artificial Life 10: 235–259.
Rummelhart, D., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J., and Hinton, G. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. McClelland, D. Rummelhart, and the PDP Research Group, Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, vol. 2, pp. 7–58. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ruse, M. (1996). The relationship between science and religion in Britain, 1830–1870. In M. Ruse, ed., But Is It Science? The Philosophical Question in the Creation/Evolution Controversy, pp. 50–70. New York: Prometheus Books.
Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibanez, V., Deiber, M. P., Dold, G., and Hallet, M. (1996). Activation of the primary visual cortex by braille reading in blind subjects. Nature 380: 526–528.
Sarkar, S. (1996). Biological information: a skeptical look at some central dogmas of molecular biology. In S. Sarkar, ed., The Philosophy and History of Molecular Biology: New Perspectives, pp. 187–232. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (2000). Information in genetics and developmental biology: comments on Maynard Smith. Philosophy of Science 67: 208–213.
Sarntheim, J., Petsche, H., Rappelsberger, P., Shaw, G. L., and von Stein, A. (1998). Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95: 7092–7096.
Sartre, J.-P. (1956). Being and Nothingness, trans. Hazel Barnes. New York: Philosophical Library.
———. (1960). The Transcendence of the Ego: An Existentialist Theory of Consciousness. New York: Noonday Press.
———. (2004). The Imaginary: A Phenomenological Psychology of the Imagination, trans. J. Webber. London: Routledge Press.
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Fields, W. M., and Taglialatela, J. P. (2001). Language, speech, tools and writing: a cultural imperative. In E. Thompson, ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, pp. 273–292. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (2001): 273–292.
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: John Wiley.
Scheerer, E. (1994). Psychoneural isomorphism: historical background and current relevance. Philosophical Psychology 7: 183–210.
Schiff, S. J., So, P., Chang, T., Burke, R. E., and Sauer, T. (1996). Detecting dynamical interdependence and generalized synchrony through mutual prediction in a neural ensemble. Physical Reviews E 54: 6708–6724.
Schmid-Schonbein, C. (1998). Improvements of seizure control by psychological methods in patients with intractable epilepsy. Seizure 7: 261–270.
Scholz, B. C. (2002). Innateness. Nature 415: 739.
Schreiber, T. (1999). Interdisciplinary application of nonlinear time series methods. Physics Reports 308: 1–40.
Schröder, J. (1998). Emergence: non-deducibility or downwards causation? Philosophical Quarterly 48: 432–452.
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
———. (1992). The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (2000a). Consciousness. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23: 557–578.
———. (2000b). Consciousness, free action and the brain. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7: 3–22.
———. (2004). Mind: A Brief Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.
Sejnowski, T., and Rosenberg, C. (1986). NETtalk: A Parellel Network that Learns to Read Aloud. Technical Report JHU/EEC-86/01. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Sellars, W. (1956). Empiricism and the philosophy of mind. In H. Feigl and M. Scriven, eds., Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Vol. 1. The Foundations of Science and the Concepts of Psychology and Psychoanalysis, pp. 253–329. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1999a). The Primacy of Movement. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Press.
———. (1999b). Emotion and movement: analysing their relationship. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 259–277.
Shepard, R., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171: 701–703.
Shimony, A. (1997). On mentality, quantum mechanics, and the actualization of potentialities. In R. Penrose, A. Shimony, N. Cartwright, and S. Hawking, The Large, the Small, and the Human Mind, pp. 144–160. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Shoemaker, S. (1968). Self-reference and self-awareness. Journal of Philosophy 65: 555–567.
———. (1984). Identity, Cause and Mind: Philosophical Essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Siewert, C. (2004). Is experience transparent? Philosophical Studies 117: 15–41.
Silberstein, M. (2001). Converging on emergence: consciousnesss, causation, and explanation. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 61–98.
———. (2002). Reduction, emergence, and explanation. In P. Machamer and M. Silberstein, eds., The Blackwell Guide to the Philosophy of Science, pp. 80–107. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Silberstein, M., and McGeever, J. (1999). The search for ontological emergence. Philosophical Quarterly 49: 182–200.
Simon, H. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics 59: 99–118.
———. (1969). The Sciences of the Artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1973). The organization of complex systems. In H. H. Pattee, ed., Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems, pp. 1–27. New York: George Braziller.
Simons, D. J., and Chabris, C. R. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28: 1059–1074.
Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1: 261–267.
Simons, D. J., and Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science 9: 315–320.
Simons, D. J., Wang, R. F., and Roddenberry, D. (2002). Object recognition is mediated by extraretinal information. Perception and Psychophysics 64: 521–530.
Skarda, C., and Freeman, W. J. (1987). How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10: 161–195.
Skuse, D. H., James, R. S., Bishop, D. V. M., Coppins, B., Dalton, P., Aamodt-Leeper, G., Bacarese-Hamilton, M., Creswell, C., McGurk, R., and Jacobs, P. A. (1997). Evidence from Turner’s Syndrome of an imprinted Xlinked locus affecting cognitive functions. Nature 387: 705–708.
Slack, J. M., Holland, P. W. H., and Graham, C. F. (1993). The zootype and the phylotypic stage. Nature 361: 490–492.
Smith, A. D. (2003). Husserl and the Cartesian Meditations. London: Routledge Press.
Smith, B. (1999). Truth and the visual field. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 317–329. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Smith, B. C. (1996). On the Origin of Objects. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Smith, K. C. (1994). The Emperor’s New Genes: The Role of the Genome in Development and Evolution. Doctoral Dissertation. Department of Philosophy, Duke University.
Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 1–74.
So, P., Francis, J. T., Netoff, T. I., Gluckman, B. J., and Schiff, S. J. (1998). Periodic orbits: a new language for neuronal dynamics. Biophysical Journal 74: 2776–2785.
Sober, E. (1993). Philosophy of Biology. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Sober, E., and Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Altruism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sokolowski, R. (2000). An Introduction to Phenomenology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Solé, R., and Goodwin, B. (2000). Signs of Life: How Complexity Pervades Biology. New York: Basic Books.
Sonea, S., and Panisset, M. (1983). A New Bacteriology. Boston: Jones and Bartlett.
Spiegelberg, H. (1994). The Phenomenological Movement: A Historical Introduction, 3d rev. and enlarged ed. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Sperry, R. (1969). A modified concept of consciousness. Psychological Review 76: 532–536.
Stawarska, B. (2001). Pictorial representation or subjective scenario? Sartre on imagination. Sartre Studies International 7: 87–111.
———. (2002). Memory and subjectivity: Sartre in dialogue with Husserl. Sartre Studies International 8: 94–111.
Stearns, S. C. (1982). On fitness. In D. Mossakowski and G. Roth, eds., Environmental Adaptation and Evolution, pp. 3–17. Suttgart: Gustav Fischer.
———. (1986). Natural selection and fitness, adaptation and constraint. In D. M. Raup and D. Jablonski, eds., Patterns and Processes in the History of Life, pp. 23–44. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
———. (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stein, E. (1989). On the Problem of Empathy, trans. Waltraut Stein. Washington, DC: ICS Publications.
Steinbock, A. J. (1995). Home and Beyond: Generative Phenomenology after Husserl. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (1999). Saturated intentionality. In Donn Welton, ed., The Body, pp. 178–199. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
———. (2004). On the phenomenology of becoming aware. Continental Philosophy Review 37: 21–43.
Sterelny, K. (1990). The imagery debate. In W. Lycan, ed., Mind and Cognition, pp. 607–626. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
———. (2000). The “genetic program” program: a commentary on Maynard Smith on information in biology. Philosophy of Science 67: 195–201.
Sterelny, K., and Griffiths, P. E. (1999). Sex and Death: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sterelny, K., and Kitcher, P. (1988). The return of the gene. Journal of Philosophy 85: 339–360.
Stern, D. N. (2000). The Interpersonal World of the Infant, 2d ed. New York: Basic Books.
———. (2004). The Present Moment in Psychotherapy and Everyday Life. New York: W. W. Norton.
Stoljar, D. (2004). The argument from diaphanousness. In M. Ezcurdia, R. Stainton, and C. Viger, eds., New Essays in the Philosophy of Language and Mind. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, supplementary vol. 30, pp. 341–390. Calgary: University of Alberta Press.
Sur, M., Angelecci, A., and Sharma, J. (1999). Rewiring cortex: the role of patterned activity in development and plasticity of neocortical circuits. Journal of Neurobiology 41: 33–43.
Swanson, L. W. (2000). What is the brain? Trends in Neurosciences 23: 519–527.
Szostak, J. W., Bartel, D. P., and Luisi, P. L. (2001). Synthesizing life. Nature 409: 387–390.
Teller, P. (1986). Relational holism and quantum mechanics. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 37: 71–81.
Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheler, C., and Smith, L. B. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: a field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 1–86.
Thelen, E., and Smith, L. B. (1994). A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Thom, R. (1975). Structural Stability and Morphogenesis. Reading, MA: W. A. Benjamin.
Thomas, L. (1974). The Lives of a Cell: Notes of a Biology Watcher. New York: Viking Press.
Thomas, N. J. T. (1999). Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content. Cognitive Science 23: 207–245.
———. (2002). The false dichotomy of imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 211.
Thompson, E. (1997). Symbol grounding: a bridge from artificial life to artificial intelligence. Brain and Cognition 34: 48–71.
———. (2001). Empathy and consciousness. In E. Thompson, ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, pp. 1–32. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (2001): 1–32.
———. (2004). Life and mind: from autopoiesis to neurophenomenology. A tribute to Francisco Varela. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 3: 381–398.
———. (2005). Empathy and human experience. In J. D. Proctor, ed., Science, Religion, and the Human Experience, pp. 261–285. New York: Oxford University Press.
Thompson, E., Lutz, A., and Cosmelli, D. (2005). Neurophenomenology: an introduction for neurophilosophers. In A. Brook and K. Akins, eds., Cognition and the Brain: The Philosophy and Neuroscience Movement, pp. 40–97. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Thompson, E., Noë, A., and Pessoa, L. (1999). Perceptual completion: a case study in phenomenology and cognitive science. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 161–195. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Thompson, E., Palacios, A., and Varela, F. J. (1992). Ways of coloring: comparative color vision as a case study for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15: 1–74. Reprinted in Alva Noë and Evan Thompson, eds., Vision and Mind: Readings in the Philosophy of Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.
Thompson, E., and Varela, F. J. (2001). Radical embodiment: neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 418–425.
Thompson, W. I. (1987a). The cultural implications of the new biology. In W. I. Thompson, ed., Gaia: A Way of Knowing. Political Implications of the New Biology, pp. 11–34. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
———, ed. (1987b). Gaia: A Way of Knowing. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
———. (1987c). Gaia and the politics of life. In W. I. Thompson, ed., Gaia: A Way of Knowing. Political Implications of the New Biology, pp. 167–214. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
———. (1989). Imaginary Landscape: Making Worlds of Myth and Science. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
———, ed. (1991a). Gaia 2: Emergence. The New Science of Becoming. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
———. (1991b). The imagination of a new science and the emergence of a planetary culture. In W. I. Thompson, ed., Gaia 2: Emergence. The New Science of Becoming, pp. 11–29. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
———. (1991c). Politics becoming planet. In W. I. Thompson, ed., Gaia 2: Emergence. The New Science of Becoming, pp. 249–269. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tononi, G., and Edelman, G. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science 282: 1846–1851.
Trevarthen, C., and Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42: 3–48.
Tsuda, I. (2001). Toward an interpretation of dynamic neural activity in terms of chaotic dynamical systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 793–847.
Turvey, M. T., and Shaw, R. E. (1999). Ecological foundations of cognition I. Symmetry and specificity of animal-environment systems. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 95–110.
Tye, M. (1991). The Imagery Debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (1995). Ten Problems of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (2000). Color, Content, and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
Uttal, W. R. (2001). The New Phrenology: The Limits of Localizing Cognitive Processes in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
van Gelder, T. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 615–665.
———. (1999a). Dynamic approaches to cognition. In R. Wilson and F. Keil, eds., The MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences, pp. 244–246. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1999b). Wooden iron: Husserlian phenomenology meets cognitive science. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 245–265. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Van Gulick, R. (2001). Reduction, emergence and other recent options on the mind-body problem: a philosophic overview. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 1–34.
VanRullen, R., and Koch, C. (2003). Is perception discrete or continuous? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 207–213.
Varela, F. J. (1979). Principles of Biological Autonomy. New York: Elsevier North Holland.
———. (1984). Living ways of sense-making: a middle path for neuroscience. In P. Livingston, ed., Disorder and Order: Proceedings of the Stanford International Symposium, pp. 208–224. Stanford Literature Series, vol. 1, Anma Libri.
———. (1987). Laying down a path in walking. In W. I. Thompson, ed., Gaia: A Way of Knowing. Political Implications of the New Biology, pp. 48–64. Hudson, NY: Lindisfarne Press.
———. (1989). Reflections on the circulation of concepts between a biology of cognition and systemic family therapy. Family Process 28: 15–24.
———. (1991). Organism: a meshwork of selfless selves. In A. Tauber, ed., Organism and the Origin of Self, pp. 79–107. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
———. (1995). Resonant cell assemblies: a new approach to cognitive functions and neuronal synchrony. Biological Research 28: 81–95.
———. (1996). Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies 3: 330–350.
———. (1997a). Patterns of life: intertwining identity and cognition. Brain and Cognition 34: 72–87.
———. (1997b). The naturalization of phenomenology as the transcendence of nature: searching for generative mutual constraints. Alter 5: 355–381.
———. (1999). The specious present: a neurophenomenology of time consciousness. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela, B. Pachoud, and J.-M. Roy, eds., Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, pp. 266–314. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
———. (2000a). El fénomena de la vida. Santiago, Chile: Dolmen Essayo.
———. (2000b). Pour une phénoménologie de la shunyata I. In N. Depraz and J.-F. Marquet, eds., La gnose: une question philosophique. Paris: Editions du Cerf.
Varela, F. J., and Bourgine, P., eds. (1991). Toward a Practice of Autonomous Systems. Proceedings of the First European Conference on Artificial Life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Varela, F. J., and Cohen, A. (1989). Le corps evocateur: une relecture de l’immunité. Nouvelle Revue de Psychanalyse 40: 193–213.
Varela, F. J., and Coutinho, A. (1991). Second generation immune networks. Immunology Today 12: 159–166.
Varela, F. J., and Depraz, N. (2003). Imagining: embodiment, phenomenology, and transformation. In B. A. Wallace, ed., Buddhism and Science: Breaking New Ground, pp. 195–232. New York: Columbia University Press.
———. (2005). At the source of time: valence and the constitutional dynamics of affect. In G. Colombetti and E. Thompson, eds., Emotion Experience, pp. 64–81. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic, 2005. Also published as a special issue of Journal of Consciousness Studies 12: 64–81.
Varela, F. J., and Goguen, J. (1978). The arithmetic of closure. In R. Trappl, ed., Progress in Cybernetics and Systems Research, vol. 3, pp. 48–63. New York: Wiley Hemisphere.
Varela, F. J., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 229–239.
Varela, F. J., Maturana, H. R., and Uribe, R. (1974). Autopoiesis: the organization of living systems, its characterization and a model. Biosystems 5: 187–196.
Varela, F. J., and Shear, J. (1999a). First-person accounts: why, what, and how. In F. J. Varela and J. Shear, eds., The View from Within: First-Person Approaches to the Study of Consciousness, pp. 1–14. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
———, eds. (1999b). The View from Within: First-Person Approaches to the Study of Consciousness. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Varela, F. J., and Thompson, E. (2003). Neural synchrony and the unity of mind: a neurophenomenological perspective. In A. Cleeremans, ed., The Unity of Consciousness: Binding, Integration and Dissociation, pp. 266–287. New York: Oxford University Press.
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vetlesen, A. J. (1994). Perception, Empathy, and Judgment: An Inquiry into the Preconditions of Moral Performance. University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press.
Virchow, R. (1967). The cell doctrine. In E. A. Carlson, ed., Modern Biology: Its Conceptual Foundations, pp. 22–25. New York: George Brazilier.
von Stein, A., Chiang, C., and König, P. (2000). Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97: 14748–14753.
von Stein, A., and Sarntheim, J. (2000). Different frequencies for different scales of cortical intergration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology 38: 301–313.
von Uexküll, J. (1957). A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In K. S. Lashley, ed., Instinctive Behavior: The Development of a Modern Concept, pp. 5–80. New York: International Universities Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Ed. M. Cole. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wake, D. B., Roth, G., and Wake, M. H. (1983). On the problem of stasis in organismal evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology 101: 211–224.
Wallace, A. B. (1998). The Bridge of Quiescence: Experiencing Tibetan Buddhist Meditation. La Salle, IL: Open Court.
———. (1999). The Buddhist tradition of shamatha: methods for refining and examining consciousness. In F. J. Varela and J. Shear, eds., The View from Within: First-Person Approaches to the Study of Consciousness, pp. 175–188. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.
Walton, K. L. (1990). Mimesis as Make-Believe: On the Foundations of the Representational Arts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Watt, D. F. (1998). Emotion and consciousness: implications of affective neuroscience for extended reticular thalamic activating system theories of consciousness. Target Article, Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness Electronic Seminar. Unpublished.
Weber, A., and Varela, F. J. (2002). Life after Kant: natural purposes and the autopoietic foundations of biological individuality. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 97–125.
Weber, B. H., and Depew, D. J. (1995). Darwinism Evolving: Systems Dynamics and the Genealogy of Natural Selection. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1996). Natural selection and self-organization. Biology and Philosophy 11: 33–65.
Webster, G., and Goodwin, B. (1996). Form and Transformation: Generative and Relational Principles in Biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Weiss, P. (1969). The living system: determinism stratified. In A. Koestler and J. R. Smythies, eds., Beyond Reductionism: New Perspectives in the Life Sciences. London: Hutchinson.
Welton, D. (1999). Soft, smooth hands: Husserl’s phenomenology of the lived body. In D. Welton, ed., The Body, pp. 38–56. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
———. (2000). The Other Husserl: The Horizons of Transcendental Phenomenology. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
———, ed. (2003). The New Husserl. A Critical Reader. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Wexler, M. and Klam, F. (2001). Movement prediction and movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27: 48–64.
Wexler, M., Kosslyn, S. M., and Berthoz, A. (1998). Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition 68: 77–94.
Wheeler, M. (1997). Cognition’s coming home: the reunion of life and mind. In P. Husbands and I. Harvey, eds., Proceedings of the 4th European Conference on Artificial Life, pp. 10–19. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Wilson, R. A. (1994). Wide computationalism. Mind 103: 351–372.
Wimsatt, W. C. (1985). Developmental constraints, generative entrenchment and the innate-acquired distinction. In W. Bechtel, ed., Integrating Scientific Disciplines, pp. 185–208. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.
———. (1986). Forms of aggregativity. In A. Donagan, A. Perovich, and M. Wedin, eds., Human Nature and Human Knowledge, pp. 259–291. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
Winograd, T., and Flores, F. (1987). Understanding Computers and Cognition. A New Foundation for Design, NJ: Ablex Press.
Wohlshläger, A., and Wohlschläger, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24: 397–412.
Wolheim, R. (1980). Art and Its Objects, 2d ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.
———. (1987). Painting as an Art. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
———. (1998). On pictorial representation. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 56: 217–226.
Yamaguchi, I. (1997). Ki als leibhaftige Vernunft. Beitrag zur interkulturellen Phänomenologie der Leiblichkeit. Munich: W. Fink Verlag.
Zahavi, D. (1997). Horizontal intentionality and transcendental intersubjectivity. Tijdschrift Voor Filosofie 59: 304–321.
———. (1999). Self-Awareness and Alterity. A Phenomenological Investigation. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
———. (2001a). Husserl and Transcendental Intersubjectivity: A Response to the Linguistic-Pragmatic Critique, trans. E. A. Benke. Athens, OH: Ohio University Press.
———. (2001b). Beyond empathy. Phenomenological approaches to intersubjectivity. In E. Thompson, ed., Between Ourselves: Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness, pp. 151–167. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic. Also published in Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 151–167.
———. (2002a). First-person thoughts and embodied self-awareness: Some reflections on the relation between recent analytical philosophy and phenomenology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 7–26.
———. (2002b). Merleau-Ponty on Husserl: a reappraisal. In T. Toadvine and L. Embree, eds., Merleau-Ponty’s Reading of Husserl, pp. 3–29. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publisher.
———. (2003a). Husserl’s Phenomenology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
———. (2003b). Inner time-consciousness and pre-reflective selfawareness. In D. Welton, ed., The New Husserl: A Critical Reader, pp. 157–180. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
———. (2003c). Intentionality and phenomenality: a phenomenological take on the hard problem. In E. Thompson (ed.), The Problem of Consciousness: New Essays in Phenomenological Philosophy of Mind. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, supplementary vol. 29, pp. 63–92. Calgary: University of Alberta Press.
———. (2004a). Husserl’s noema and the internalism-externalism debate. Inquiry 47: 42–66.
———. (2004b). Phenomenology and the project of naturalization. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 3: 331–347.
———. (2005a). Subjectivity and Selfhood: Investigating the First-Person Perspective. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/A Bradford Book.
———. (2005b). Being someone. Psyche 11 (5), June 2005. URL: http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/symposia/metzinger/Zahavi.pdf
Zahavi, D., and Parnas, J. (1998). Phenomenal consciousness and selfawareness: a phenomenological critique of representational theory. Journal of Consciousness Studies 5: 687–705.
Zaretzky, A. N., and Letelier, J. C. (2002). Metabolic networks from (M,R) systems and autopoiesis perspective. Journal of Biological Systems 10: 265–284.