Chapter 7
In This Chapter
Understanding how to grab someone’s attention
Controlling attention when you have it
Digging into attention disorders
Consider the environment around you while reading this book. We doubt that it’s absolutely quiet. Most likely you can hear distractions (perhaps a TV, music, the sound of someone vacuuming). Yet, despite these distractions, you can remain (mostly) focused on what you’re reading. The huge amount of sensory input that everyone is constantly receiving would be overwhelming if humans weren’t able to block out some of it. The psychological mechanism of this filtering and focusing ability is attention.
Attention acts like a spotlight, a focusing device to bring to mind a particular stimulus. It also acts as a filter, blocking out distractions.
In this chapter, we describe the key features of what attracts attention, what doesn’t attract attention (even when it probably should!) and how attention helps you search for things. We also discuss how to control attention, the mechanics of involuntary attention and some clinical disorders of attention.
Understanding attention is essential in many areas of life. One group that relies on it to make a living are magicians.
In 2008, the prestigious journal Nature Reviews Neuroscience published an article by a number of magicians (including Teller of Penn and Teller fame) explaining how they use knowledge of human cognition to execute their tricks and how this knowledge can help cognitive psychologists understand how the brain works. One sure common skill is misdirection – guiding the audience’s attention away from the real trick. Misdirection involves several aspects of attention that we define and discuss in this section:
Expectation can also be used for priming. American psychologist Michael Posner developed a test called the Posner cueing task, which measures how the attention system responds to different cues.
Participants are presented with a fixation cross (a ‘+’) in the middle of the screen. Then a cue appears, directing their attention to one or the other side of the fixation cross. A target appears, and participants have to respond to the target (for example, by saying what shape it is). The cue can be valid or invalid. On valid trials, the cue predicts the location of the target. On invalid trials, the cue doesn’t predict the location of the target. Neutral trials, in which no cue exists, are also used.
Participants were presented with cross shapes and had to identify whether the horizontal bar was longer than the vertical bar. The crosses were on screen for only 200 milliseconds; participants were looking at a fixation cross, not looking directly at the cross shape. People were highly accurate at this task. But when researchers replaced the fixation point with a shape (a triangle, rectangle or cross), 86 per cent of participants didn’t notice the change.
To produce change blindness, psychologists need to mask these transients. They can do so in several ways:
Daniel J. Simons offers these explanations for change blindness:
The popular Where’s Wally books (Where’s Waldo in the US) involve trying to find the eponymous hero in a distinctive outfit in a cluttered scene. The task is difficult because the scene contains lots of other people and objects, and many of them share colours and features similar to the target. Cognitive psychologists use a more formal version of this task called the visual search task to understand the role of attention in vision.
In both displays in Figure 7-1a, the B shares the same features (vertical and curved lines) as the distractors P, so it doesn’t pop out. You have to check each item in turn, searching for the specific conjunction of features, so you take longer to find the target in the top image because of more distractors. In Figure 7-1b, the O is the only shape containing curved lines, so it pops out and the number of distractors has little effect.
According to psychologist Anne Treisman’s feature integration theory, attention is the ‘glue’ that binds the features together in visual search. In pre-attentive search, your visual system picks out features (such as colour, shape, size and movement). At this stage, if the target is the only item containing a certain feature, it pops out. But if the target depends on a conjunction of features, it doesn’t pop out and you have to look for it, one item at a time. By attending to an object, you’re able to ‘glue’ the different features together.
Implicit in the definition of attention is the idea that you can take control of your conscious experience. Attention shines like a spotlight on a stimulus (or stimuli). It raises awareness of key parts of the thing you’re attending to while dimming others. It selects what reaches your consciousness.
Imagine, for example, that you’re revising for a cognitive psychology exam (reading this book, of course, and writing notes), while the TV’s on, someone’s hollering about dinner and you’re expecting a text message. Attention removes the distraction and focuses on the key task (revising) or tasks (revising and listening out for your mobile phone).
In this section, we cover how you choose (or not) what you attend to. We also describe what happens when you have multiple things to attend to at once and how difficult that is. Plus we explore what factors push your attentional capacity to the limit.
The first thing your brain needs to do when studying is to select the relevant things to attend to: in other words, on what to put the attentional spotlight (or unidirectional microphone for sounds!). Here we look at how the brain selects information to attend to.
In other dichotic listening tasks, participants have to shadow one ear but ignore the other. They’re then asked to recall information from the attended ear (in which the message was shadowed) and the unattended ear (the one they were supposed to ignore). Participants are nearly perfect at remembering information from the attended ear, but remember very little from the unattended ear. In fact, participants don’t notice whether the language used in the unattended ear swaps from English to German or even if the same word is repeated several dozen times!
Participants do notice, however, if the gender of the speaker in the unattended ear changes. Furthermore, most participants are unaware of an instruction to stop the task in the unattended ear unless it’s preceded by their name. Colin Cherry, a British cognitive psychologist, described this tendency as the cocktail party effect – even if you’re attending to something else, you sometimes hear your name spoken.
Some researchers use the cocktail party effect to criticise the early-selection theory. Specifically, one study criticised the theory by giving mild electric shocks to participants every time a particular word was presented in the unattended ear. This formed a classical conditioning pairing of a word and a shock. When asked to recall the words, participants couldn’t recall the word paired with the shock. But when shown the word, they had a higher skin galvanic response (their hands got a little sweatier), suggesting that they were mildly afraid of the word. This suggests that they had attended to and memorised the word, but not consciously.
Anne Triesman developed the attenuation model in which attention simply reduces the amount of information that can get through the filter. The early filter still blocks out unwanted stimulation, but allows information through that has certain physical properties. Diana Deutsch, British-American perceptual and cognitive psychologist, went further, and suggested that all information is processed and attention simply filters out the unwanted information and the semantic (meaning) level (known as late selection). We present these theories in Figure 7-3.
In an experiment on multitasking, participants shadowed one list of words from one ear while ignoring a second list presented verbally or visually. The conditions for list 1 and list 2 were respectively: spoken words/spoken words; spoken words/visual words. Researchers tested recall for both lists and accuracy was higher for the spoken words/visual words condition than when the lists were matched across senses. The message is clear: you can divide attention across multiple tasks as long as they’re dissimilar enough.
Although people can multitask a little (see the preceding section), limits apply. If you’re in a room full of noisy people who’re moving around and discussing what they watched on TV the night before, planning an essay in cognitive psychology is nearly impossible. You simply have too much sensory information to filter out.
Research shows that executive control allocates attentional resources from one task to another and inhibits automatic responses. Cognitive psychologists have found that executive control is strongly related to tests of inhibition. Executive control also correlates with intelligence, suggesting that more intelligent people can allocate their attention more appropriately than less intelligent people.
Your attention can be under voluntary control – such as when you decide to ignore your ringing phone to carry on watching your favourite TV programme. But your attention can also be automatic – everyone has experienced their attention being drawn to something because of a loud noise or an unusual display. Sometimes, people simply work on ‘autopilot’ – for example, one of us (no names, no blame!) accidentally travelled to college one day instead of to an interview, because that was his normal morning routine and he was too sleepy to prevent the automatic behaviour.
Psychologists have used a number of tools to investigate why this happens. In this section, we cover what factors can interfere with attention and make it worse. We also examine the important effects practice has on attention.
Finished files are the result of years of scientific study combined with the experience of many years.
The most common answer is three but the correct answer is six. Many people seem to miss the ‘f’s when they occur in the word ‘of’.
Participants find the incongruent condition much harder and take longer to name the colours than with the congruent condition: reading is so automatic that psychologists assume that the word’s meaning disrupts people’s ability to name the colour.
Driving is an obvious example. Novice drivers tend to pay more attention to the car in front of them and its position than expert drivers do. But the latter tend to pay more attention to cars to the side or at junctions (in positions where cars may do unexpected things, such as pull out suddenly).
One suggestion is that practice makes a task more automatic, and so it requires less attentional resources. Executive control is primarily needed to inhibit automatic processing or when automatic processes are unavailable. So, when a behaviour is well practised, it becomes habit and, as a result, you don’t think about the action.
When you’re learning the chord of C on the guitar, you have to do four steps: you need to position your forefinger on the b-string, your second finger on the d-string, your ring finger on the a-string, and then strum on all but the e-string. Eric Clapton, however, can chunk all these small aspects into one instruction: ‘play the chord of C’. The cognitive resources to make four responses are simplified into making one response.
Belgium psychologist Bruno Rossion suggests that experts have a wider attentional spotlight, allowing them to see more of a stimuli in one glance than a novice.
Attention is crucial for human survival. Yet neuropsychological conditions exist in which the ability to attend is severely impaired. In this section, we take a look at two attention disorders: spatial neglect and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Don’t worry, despite the heading this section isn’t going to get all political! Spatial neglect is a relatively common disorder of attention usually caused by damage to the right parietal lobe (a bit of the brain towards the side and back of the head). Patients suffering from spatial neglect appear not to see or attend to half of the visual field (usually the left side – the opposite side to the damage). That is, they can see only what’s on the right side of an object.
In all cases, patients can’t detect what’s on the neglected side. Things can be so severe that patients even shave only one side of their face or eat half the food on their plate. Patients with spatial neglect can’t visualise, imagine or even describe the neglected side of a place. In one study, a patient with neglect was asked to imagine and describe a famous square in Milan. The person described the features on the right-hand side of the square. When asked to imagine and describe the square from the opposite side, the person again described the right-hand side (the opposite side from before). Clearly, someone with neglect doesn’t have a problem with perception, but attention.
Although patients with neglect seem unable to attend to one side, they’re unconsciously aware of the neglected side. British neuropsychologists John Marshall and Peter Halligan conducted a study in which patient PS was given two pictures: house A was a normal line drawing (see Figure 7-4c); house B was identical, except that it had flames coming out of the upper window on the neglected side. PS was asked which house she preferred. On 15 out of 17 trials, PS chose the one without the flames but was unable to explain her preference.
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common childhood psychological disorder. It occurs in 3–5 per cent of Western children and is characterised by an inability to focus or maintain attention for a long time, leading to restlessness and potentially aggression. Children with ADHD often interrupt others and are impatient.
Another experiment asks participants to respond with a button press when they see an X and a different key when they see an O. Children with ADHD perform well at this easy task. An instruction to stop the response when a sound is played at the same time as the X or O occurs on a few trials (the stop-signal paradigm). Children with ADHD are less likely to stop on these trials than other children, showing that children with ADHD have problems inhibiting their responses.
One treatment for ADHD is the stimulant methylphenidate, which makes the brain more responsive. The idea is that the brains of children with ADHD need more stimulation to show the same activation as children without ADHD. Methylphenidate makes children with ADHD perform better at the stop-signal experiment and makes them better able to inhibit.