CHAPTER 5

IGNORING THE SIGNPOSTS

A Process Perspective of
Unethical and Destructive Leadership


Diane J. Chandler and Dail Fields

This chapter examines how salient dimensions of leaders, followers, and organizations interact within organizational processes to enable unethical and destructive leadership behavior. Three questions frame this discussion: (a) how and why did leaders venture down paths of destructive behaviors?, (b) why did these leaders persist in behaviors that most knew or would identify as unethical and destructive?, and (c) why were these behaviors tolerated, and in some instances rewarded, by organizations and followers? This chapter assesses how social-cognitive tendencies operating within organizational processes affect destructive leadership behavior through leadership selection, self-enhancement motives, escalation of commitment, threat-rigidity response, organizational commitment, feedback-seeking behaviors and feedback environment, and pressures for conformity. Six propositions derive from a process model for unethical and destructive leadership.

INTRODUCTION

Research in organizational studies has presented alternative conceptualizations of negative, unethical, and destructive leadership, as well as the consequences of these leader behaviors (Einarsen, Aasland, & Skogstad, 2007; Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 2007; Schilling, 2009). Destructive leadership has been defined as repeated behaviors that work against the legitimate interests of an organization and its members including the undermining the organization’s goal, resources, and quality of working life (Einarsen et al., 2007). Destructive leadership may involve dominance, coercion, manipulation of others, and selfish leader-focused orientation (Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007). While previous studies have considered leader and follower characteristics that may be associated with the occurrence of destructive leadership, organizations succeed or fail based on the effectiveness and resilience of their processes for strategy and human resource development (Collins, 2001; Pfeffer & Sutton, 2006). As a result, antecedents of destructive leadership must be considered within the contexts of organizational processes. The purpose of this chapter is to examine how certain key variables of leaders, followers, and organizations may interact within organizational processes to enable unethical and destructive leadership.

In a recent qualitative study, Schilling (2009) found that only 25% of the identified variables accounting for negative leadership behaviors were attributable to individual leader characteristics, whereas 75% were attributed to situational variables. Leadership characteristics alone do not fully account for why leaders behave destructively. In most organizational settings, situational variables are strong determinants of employee behaviors. If followers in an organization observe leaders’ disregard of honesty, integrity, and ethical behavior, the minimum result may be confusion and an implied directive that such behaviors are an acceptable norm, setting the stage for social pressure to exhibit unethical, negative, and possibly destructive behaviors (Bazerman & Benaji, 2004; Brass, Butterfield, & Skaggs, 1998; Schweitzer & Gibson, 2008). Therefore examining organizational processes and the related outcomes are important for understanding how destructive leadership behavior may arise and be tolerated in organizational settings (Brown & Treviño, 2006a; Manz, Anand, Joshi, & Manz, 2008; Padilla et al., 2007; Schilling, 2009).

In explaining unethical and destructive leadership behavior, researchers have focused primarily on the characteristics of (a) leaders (Benson & Hogan, 2008; Dotlich & Cairo, 2003, Kellerman, 2004; Kets de Vries, 1993, 2006), (b) followers (Lord & Brown, 2004; Offerman, 2004), (c) environmental variables (Schilling, 2009; Zimbardo, 2006, 2007), or (d) a conflux of leader, follower and environmental conditions (Padilla et al., 2007; Popper, 2001; Rhode, 2006; Vardi & Weitz, 2004). However as we review and observe examples of destructive organizational leaders whose unethical and sometimes malicious behaviors have come to light, we are confronted by three penetrating questions related to why organizations and their members ignore the obvious signposts, the tell-tale signs of unethical and destructive leadership actions:

  • How and why did such leaders venture down paths of such destructive behaviors?
  • Why did these leaders persist in behaviors that most knew or would identify as unethical and destructive?
  • Why were these behaviors tolerated, and in some instances rewarded, by organizations and followers?

The focus of this chapter is to address these three questions by examining how characteristics of leaders, followers, and organizations interact, while highlighting the organizational processes seldom explored in relationship to unethical and destructive leadership. Specifically we examine the effects of cognitive tendencies operating in organizational processes such as (a) self-enhancement motives (Pfeffer & Fong, 2005), (b) escalation of commitment (Street & Street, 2006), (c) threat-rigidity response (Staw, Sandelands, & Dutton, 1981), (d) organizational commitment (Peterson, 2003), (e) feedback-seeking behaviors and the feedback environment (Herold & Parsons, 1985; Janssen & Prins, 2007; Whitaker, Dahling, & Levy, 2007), and (f) pressures for conformity (Zimbardo, 1969, 2006, 2007).

Although unethical leadership behavior may not immediately eventuate in destructive organizational outcomes, the seeds of destruction have been sown despite whether or not unethical behaviors ever come to light, creating a desensitizing effect upon ethical values, standards, and conduct. Therefore, a fundamental premise of this chapter is that unethical leadership behavior is ultimately destructive in nature. Hence, we link the terms unethical and destructive leadership in tandem.

Figure 5.1 presents a model showing the linkages among the major organizational processes analyzed in this chapter.

1. WHY DO LEADERS VENTURE DOWN UNETHICAL AND DESTRUCTIVE PATHS?

In addressing the first of three major questions, we begin by examining the leadership selection process itself involving common selection biases. Some have argued that executive leaders are chosen to address short-term fixes rather than long-term success (Hogan & Kaiser, 2008), while others pinpoint the lack of systematically applied and valid selection criteria (DeVries, 1992), including demonstrated character and integrity (Treviño, Hartman, & Brown, 2000).

e9781617350696_i0006.jpg

Figure 5.1. A process perspective of destrutive leadership.

1.1. Oversights in the Leadership Selection Process

The fundamental area of leadership selection is a relevant topic when considering the unethical and destructive behavior of leaders. The effects of the methods by which leaders are selected have not been explored extensively or empirically in the leadership literature (De Cremer & van Dijk, 2008). These methods may be laden with bias. Bias in leadership selection derives from the attraction-selection-attrition (ASA) framework (Schneider, 1987). The ASA process suggests that selection decisions tend to reflect in-group bias based on perceived similarities of candidates to existing occupants of organizational positions of power. This tendency operates, in part, due to a confirmation bias known as the “halo effect,” or the perception that positive qualities in one area apply to another (Cooper, 1981; Meindl & Erlich, 1987). Schneider, Goldstein, and Smith (1995) cautioned that leadership selection based exclusively on similarity-attraction may lead to over-homogenization that limits organizational flexibility and performance, producing an in-group bias. However, other empirical results suggested that perceived perceptions of threats or danger may alter the in-group focus to perceived tendencies toward more decisive, directive leaders (Hoyt, Simon, & Reid, 2009). If those making leadership selections tend to view current environmental conditions as threatening, there is an increased likelihood that candidates perceived as more directive and dominant may be preferred. While a dominant personality alone may not signal a tendency toward unethical or destructive behaviors, powerful personalities combined with perceived organizational threats may be a toxic combination (Padilla et al., 2007).

Observing that most executive searches are conducted without a systematic assessment process, Harshman and Harshman (2008) noted, “there seems to be a general feeling among boards and search firm executives that asking top level (Board of Directors, CEO, or president) candidates to participate in an assessment is demeaning and will drive good candidates away” (p. 186). Others advocate the use of formal cognitive and leadership assessments to evaluate leadership skills and technical ability (Hogan, 2006; Hogan, Curphy, & Hogan, 1994; Hogan, Raskin, & Fazzini, 1990). For example in a meta-analysis, Judge, Colbert, and Ilies (2004) found that the relationship between intelligence and leadership effectiveness, although significant, was lower than expected. Perceived intelligence was more strongly associated with leadership than intelligence measured objectively with tests. In addition, consistent with cognitive resource theory, the relationship of intelligence with leadership effectiveness declined significantly when leaders were under greater stress. Therefore, traditional intelligence assessments may not predict how leaders will interact in decision-making situations when under pressure. However as Padilla and Mulvey (2008) observed, most leadership selection measures assess productive job behaviors, not counterproductive ones, creating an imbalanced evaluative procedure.

Although intelligence, personality, world view, and style may be observable, other markers, such as character, integrity, and ethical standards, albeit less observable, remain vitally important to assess. In particular, “dark side” characteristics of leaders, which may underlie unethical and destructive behavior, are difficult to detect through assessments because candidates with strong social skills and a positive self-esteem demeanor may camouflage these characteristics (Hogan et al., 1994). In addition, a candidate may present several attributes that are highly consistent with the key characteristics within implicit leadership models used by the organizational selectors. However, as the “halo effect” suggests, there is a tendency to associate the presence of a few key attributes with the presence of others that may be unrelated. For example in their study of retail store managers, Porr and Fields (2006) found leadership success on an attribute considered important by different observers resulted in vastly different assessments of the same managers. Since most leadership selectors have little experience or expertise in the selection process, the role of their implicit leadership assumptions may be a key determinant in leader selection decisions. Without realizing it, leadership selectors may be unknowingly hiring those with hidden “dark side” characteristics that might later sabotage personal and organizational integrity.

As De Cremer and van Dijk (2008) noted, assigning the label of “leader” can make two possible competing individual motives salient. On one hand, being selected as a leader may motivate a person to model socially responsible behavior; on the other, selection as a leader may trigger feelings of personal entitlement to receive special privileges or rewards. Experimental results have shown that followers are more accepting of appointed leaders taking larger shares of available rewards and that appointed leaders exhibit lower levels of social responsibility than elected leaders (De Cremer & van Dijk, 2008). However, the latter differences disappeared when the appointed leaders demonstrated social responsibility and ethical behavior. Sessa and Kaiser (2000) observed that the likelihood of hiring unethical and destructive leaders may be reduced when selectors consult others, work as a team, add leadership selection criteria such as demonstrated integrity (Padilla & Mulvey, 2008; Singh, 2008), and emphasize expectations for leaders to demonstrate social responsibility (De Cremer & van Dijk, 2008; De Hoogh & Hartog, 2008). Unethical and destructive leadership might well be mitigated with carefully crafted leadership selection criteria and processes.

1.2. Leader Characteristics

As mentioned, certain personal characteristics of candidates for leadership positions can be readily measured with available standardized assessment instruments and procedures. Other attributes of leaders are more difficult to assess because standardized instruments do not readily exist or because organizations are unwilling to put candidates through an arduous assessment process. However, some of these underassessed leader attributes may be critical indicators of attributes that may lead to unethical and destructive leadership. These attributes include LOC, narcissism, degree of cognitive moral development, and responsibility disposition. These specific four characteristics have been selected for inclusion, as they have been found in the literature to impact the decision-making process and contribute to unethical and destructive leadership.

1.2.1. Locus of Control (LOC)

LOC is identified as a personality attribute that describes how individuals perceive their ability to control events and make attributions about causality (Lefcourt, 1966; Phares, 1976; Rotter, 1966). Those with an internal LOC surmise that they are able to influence events and leadership results based on their ability and skill. Those with an external LOC believe that circumstances outside of themselves are the primary causes for events and their leadership behavior. Researchers have found that managers with an internal LOC were more confident in their ability to control events, coped better with challenging situations, but also opted for riskier strategies in order to advance organizational performance (Anderson, 1977; Miller & Toulouse, 1986; Miller, Kets de Vries, & Toulouse, 1982). Treviño and Youngblood (1990) found individuals with greater internal LOC made more ethical decisions. Leaders with more external LOC were more likely to attribute causes to external conditions, possibly leading to their denial of personal responsibility for destructive behaviors and related outcomes. Leaders higher in self-regulation are more likely to control behaviors and decisions and may be less prone to unethical or destructive behaviors (Moss, Dowling, & Callanan, 2009).

Since the essence of self-regulation is a heightened awareness of self and self-responsibility, it seems likely that external LOC and self-regulation are negatively related. Street and Street (2006) found that those with an external LOC were more likely than those with an internal LOC to select unethical options related to decision-making, suggesting less personal self-regulation. While not necessarily leading directly to destructive leadership behaviors, LOC may impact leaders’ interactions with others through role-taking within organizational processes. LOC may affect the way that leaders make attributions and interact with others in organizational processes. Frankly, externals may be more likely to cave to external pressures, while internals may resist regulation. Both tendencies could lead to destructive behaviors worth noting in consideration of the way leaders act within organizational processes. While one would expect a difference in LOC related to persistence in achieving ends (i.e., internals have more persistence than externals), studies have found that there is no significant difference between internals and externals related to persistence (Littig & Sanders, 1979; Pittenger, 2002; Starnes & Zinzer, 1983) and that the effects of LOC may be both context-specific and related to self-efficacy, interpersonal control, and sociopolitical control (Paulus & Christie, 1981).

1.2.2. Narcissism

A second pertinent leader characteristic is narcissism, a personality trait often associated with egotism, self-interest, grandiosity, arrogance, and entitlement (Kets de Vries, 2006; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006). With failure rates among senior executives cited as between 50-60% and the selection process seldom assessing personality (Hogan et al., 1994), narcissism becomes an important variable to consider. For example, narcissistic leaders take more credit for their successes, blame others for their failures, are disposed to lapses in judgment and conduct, and may be abusive (Hogan et al., 1990; Kramer, 2003; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006). Although narcissistic leaders may possess the capacity to attract followers (Kohut, 1966; Maccoby, 2003), the weaknesses inherent to this personality trait can lead directly to unethical behavior through influences on leader actions within organizational processes (Kets de Vries & Miller, 1997; Padilla et al., 2007). Narcissistic leaders may pass selection screening because they possess strengths such as the ability to present a vision in a charismatic fashion, inspire others with rhetoric, and thereby persuade others to follow (Maccoby, 2000). With pressures to hire for competitive advantage, executives charged with selecting leaders may focus on these positive characteristics. However, these strengths may camouflage leaders’ underlying self-focus, lack of empathy, desire to control others, distaste for collaboration, and an intense desire to win at all costs. Narcissistic leaders’ faults may become more pronounced if they succeed and as their personal need for power nears fulfillment (Maccoby, 2003; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006). Characterized by independence, arrogance, and inordinate need for recognition, narcissists risk intentional isolation to protect their perceived superiority and frequently exhibit amorality and/or moral myopia where “the ends justify the means;” and followers are expected to provide blind support (Rhode, 2006; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006).

Narcissism may develop in childhood after not receiving the caring support needed for healthy psycho-social development. Attributed to damage in the formative “mirroring” process between infant and mother where the child is deprived in some way, this reactive narcissism creates a yearning for positive affirmation and individuation in later adult stages (Kohut, 1971; Pines, 1981; Popper, 2001). These feelings of inferiority may manifest in self-protective defense mechanisms leading to overcompensation (Jordan, Spencer, Zanna, Hoshino-Brown, & Correll, 2003; Ziglar-Hill, 2006), self-enhancement (John & Robins, 1994), and a compulsive need for recognition (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001). These effects of narcissism may manifest in intimidation of others involved in regulatory processes within organizations limiting the ability of these processes to curb unethical and destructive behaviors of leaders.

1.2.3. Cognitive Moral Development (CMD)

Third, leaders’ lack of cognitive moral development (CMD) contributes to destructive leadership and decision making (Treviño, 1986, Treviño & Youngblood, 1990; Treviño, Hartman, & Brown, 2000). Kohlberg (1969, 1984) suggested that moral development advances from obedience motives and fear of punishment (stage 1), to reciprocity exchange (stage 2), to deciding issues of right and wrong based on expectations of others (stage 3), to abiding by rules and laws (stage 4), to internalized standards (stage 5), and then to universally held principles of justice and rights (stage 6) (Kohlberg, Levine, & Hewer, 1983). Stages 5 and 6 are associated with adulthood. Cognitive moral development has been shown to predict ethical decision making (Trevino & Youngblood, 1990). Building on previous models (Kohlberg, 1969, 1984; Rest, 1979, 1986; Trevino, 1986), Jones (1991) further argued that ethical decision making is contingent on the moral intensity of the subject issue, defining moral intensity as “the extent of issue-related moral imperative in a situation” (p. 372). Moral intensity involves the magnitude of the consequences of a decision, probability of effect, temporal immediacy, proximity, and concentration of effort. In an empirical study of ethical decision making, Paolillo and Vitell (2002) found that moral intensity accounted for significant portions of the variance in ethical decision making over other variables including job satisfaction and organizational commitment. It appears that tendencies towards unethical and destructive leadership depend not only on the level of leader moral development, but also the interaction of leaders’ moral development with the moral intensity of choices they must make as leaders. It is also likely that individual moral development may affect the nature and intensity of interactions among leader and others within organizational dynamics. Leaders with lower levels of moral development may pull off some “wins” in competitive situations that help insulate destructive leadership behaviors from scrutiny and regulation.

1.2.4. Responsibility Disposition

A fourth leader characteristic relates to responsibility disposition. Pearce, Manz, and Sims (2008) suggested that leaders may be differentiated by their personalized, as compared to socialized, power orientation. The key distinction between these types of power orientations can be measured by the leader’s responsibility disposition. Leaders with higher levels of socialized need for power tend to have a high responsibility disposition, while leaders with higher personalized need for power tend to have a low responsibility disposition. Mumford, Helton, Decker, Connelly, and Van Doorn (2003) found that individuals whose values stressed contributions to others, as opposed to personal gain, exhibited greater integrity in making decisions. Accordingly, these leaders may restrain use of power and may be less likely to use power for personal gain. Overall, this research suggests that leaders that have greater regard for socialized power may be less likely to engage in unethical and destructive behavior. In contrast, leaders with a greater need for personalized power may be lower in responsibility disposition and utilize power solely for personal gain (Hogan et al., 1994). Such leaders may persuasively stress the importance of personal allegiance to themselves as leaders, as opposed to loyalty to the overall organization. Compared to other characteristics of leaders considered in selection and promotion processes, LOC, narcissism, cognitive moral development, and responsibility disposition are formidable antecedents that affect leader performance in organizational processes and may thus indirectly make it difficult for organizations to regulate or “rein in” destructive leader behaviors.

Proposition 1. Leader selection processes are likely to overlook underlying leader characteristics such as LOC, narcissism, level of moral development, and responsibility disposition which may affect leader actions in organizational processes and limit the effects of regulatory safeguards to curb destructive leadership behaviors.

1.3. Organizational Characteristics and Processes

To address the question as to how and why leaders venture down unethical and destructive paths, we suggest that both internal and external organizational characteristics and processes contribute to creating ripe environments for such behavior (Treviño, 1986). Three factors impacting organizational context include ethical climate, goal setting pressure, and intense competition.

1.3.1. Ethical Climate

We maintain that ethical climate is a factor contributing to unethical and destructive leadership behavior. Schneider’s (1975) commonly accepted definition of organizational climate relates to perceptions that “psychologically meaningful molar [environmental] descriptions that people can agree characterize a system’s practices and procedures” (p. 474). In other words, organizational climate is fostered by members’ shared perceptions related to the organizational practices and procedures within psychological environments (Reicher & Schneider, 1990). Victor and Cullen (1988) proposed that ethical climates vary along two dimensions, namely ethical criteria relating to ethical decisions based on egoism, benevolence, or principle; and the focus of ethical reasoning relating to the scope of ethical decisions. With the ethical dimension of organizational climate impacting job satisfaction and performance (Cullen, Parbo-teeah, & Victor, 2003), we argue that when ethical climates exhibit weak ethical criteria and the focus of ethical reasoning is minimal, that unethical and destructive behaviors of leaders are more likely.

For example, in their empirical study, Schminke, Ambrose, and New-baum (2005) examined the impact of leader moral development upon ethical climate. Sampling 269 participants representing 47 firms, they found that moral development of leaders was strongly correlated with an ethical organizational climate. Second, they found that the relationship between leader moral development and ethical climate was stronger in younger versus older organizations in four out of five ethical climate types, which seemed to suggest that leaders’ ethical values, as they apply to organizational development, would be less hindered with fewer levels of bureaucratic and/or organizational structure. Third, results affirmed that value congruence between leaders and employees heightened job satisfaction and commitment, while reducing turnover intentions. These findings affirm that leader values and moral development impact followers’ shared assumptions (Schein, 1992), which in turn influence the overall ethical climate. An possible outgrowth of organizational climate is goal-setting pressure.

1.3.2. Goal Setting Pressure

When organizational pressures mount for goal accomplishment, the likelihood of unethical and destructive behaviors may likewise increase. After reviewing nearly 400 goal-setting studies, Locke and Latham (1990) found that the vast majority of these studies focused on the beneficial effects of performance due to setting specific, challenging goals. However, a study investigating Lewicki’s (1983) model of deception found that those with unmet specific goals were more likely to engage in unethical behavior than individuals attempting to “do their best” (Schweitzer, Ordonnez, & Douma, 2004). Lewicki also found that the relationship between specific goal setting and unethical behavior was particularly strong when people fell just short of reaching established goals. In the process, leaders may employ deception to protect themselves in order to frame their goal achievement in positivistic terms.

Often, self-deception, the paradoxical capacity to deceive oneself regarding the truth in the process of protecting self-image, may involve evasive mechanisms including willful ignorance, systematic ignoring, emotional detachment, self-pretense, and rationalization. A decision to deceive others may be based on decision makers’ perceptions of the costs and benefits of this course of action (Schweitzer et al., 2004). In particular, Lewicki (1983) speculated that people underestimate the costs of being deceptive because of self-justification. It appears, then, that the act of goal setting may alter perceptions of the benefits of engaging in unethical behavior. That is, leaders’ setting or accepting specific challenging goals may increase their perceptions of the benefits to be gained, in contrast to the costs, in undertaking unethical behaviors (Schweitzer et al., 2004). Likewise, Barsky (2008) argued that performance goals may intercept employees’ ethical sensibilities and disengage their internal moral and social controls (Bandura, 1999), by predicting that increasingly more difficult and specific goals increases the likelihood of unethical behavior. Goal setting pressures and goal achievement may contribute to leaders travelling down paths of unethical and destructive behavior.

1.3.3. Intense Competition

Competitive and unstable environments may place high levels of pressure on organizations to retain market share, financial viability, and dominance. When environments are unstable and unpredictable, the lack of previous precedents for how to make ethical organizational decisions causes organizational leaders to rely on their own personal ethical codes to guide behaviors. Organizational instability may result from shortage of resources, escalating costs, downsizing, mergers, and global factors such as economic recession, which are beyond the organization’s and leader’s control. A 2005 survey of over 1,000 executives and managers in global business found that 70% of the participants indicated that pressure to meet unrealistic business objectives and deadlines was the most prevalent reason leaders cited in explaining unethical behaviors (American Management Association, 2006). Pressure to meet targets within the constraints of time and resource allocation continues to plague organizational life, which provides greater conditions for ethical violations in the pursuit of survival (Webley & Warner, 2008). Given that leaders may venture down paths of unethical and destructive behaviors, the question arises as to why they persist in these behaviors in light of the potential negative outcomes, which could sabotage their credibility and the viability of their respective organizations.

Proposition 2. Organizational characteristics such as the prevailing ethical climate, pressure to set aggressive goals, and intense competition will combine to limit the impact that organizational processes may have on regulating destructive leader behaviors.

2. WHY DO LEADERS PERSIST IN UNETHICAL AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIORS?

Most leaders confront challenges in their leadership roles related to uncertainty, priorities, decision-making, and interpersonal conflicts, which may challenge or reinforce personal value alignment. Whereas in some situations leaders may be confronted with options to behave unethically, self-monitoring may dissuade them from doing so (Bandura, 1997).

In other cases, leaders may persist in unethical and destructive behaviors. This section specifically addresses leader and organizational characteristics /processes in an attempt to explain the reasons for leaders’ persistence in unethical and destructive behavior.

2.1. Leader Characteristics

Leadership characteristics are salient variables in explaining unethical and destructive leadership behavior. We focus on characteristics that have received less attention in the research literature including personal values and the self-enhancement motive, escalation of commitment, and self-deception.

2.1.1. Personal Values and the Self-Enhancement Motive

The ethical values of leaders form a cornerstone of decision making, conduct, and ethical leadership (Cameron, 2003; Heath, 2002; Quinn, 2003). As such, leaders’ values create internal pressure to behave in a prescribed way, often in an effort to align behaviors with values (Illies & Reiter-Palmon, 2008; Rokeach, 1973). In other words, individual values are predictive of leader behavior. For example, Bass and Steidlmeier (1998) argued that an absence of honesty, fairness, and a disregard for justice may lead to misdirected and potentially destructive leadership behavior. Furthermore, individual leader values strongly influence the behavior of organizational members and the way that organizational values are implemented (Grojean, Resick, Dickson, & Smith, 2004; Meglino & Ravlin, 1998).

Schwartz (1992, 1994) developed and tested a value framework that incorporates 10 values that are related in a circumflex. The Schwartz value types include (a) self-direction, (b) stimulation, (c) hedonism, (d) achievement, (e) power, (f) security, (g) conformity, (h) tradition, (i) benevolence, and (j) universalism. These 10 values can also be aggregated into four major dimensions of self-enhancement, self-transcendence, conservatism, and openness to change. Applying Schwartz’s value theory, Illies and Reiter-Palmons (2008) found that participants with dominant self-enhancement values were more destructive in leadership situations than participants with self-transcendence values.

Originating from social psychology (Fiske, 2004) and social identity perspectives (Tajfel & Turner, 1986), self-enhancement theory predicts that persons high in self-enhancement tend to see themselves and their behaviors in the most positive light (Pfeffer & Fong, 2005) and ignore or avoid situations which expose unflattering information (Sedikides & Green, 2000). Sedikides (1993) found the self-enhancement motive as the dominant factor in self-evaluation processes over other variables, and Sedikides, Gaertner, and Toguchi (2003) identified self-enhancement as a universal human motive. Overall, leaders’ self-enhancement motives tend to prompt self-serving social judgments (Beauregard & Dunning, 1998) and distort self-assessments (Dunning, Meyerowitz, & Holzberg, 1989). Leaders with strong self-enhancement motives tend to favor others who are similar to themselves by establishing in-groups, which then reinforces social identity with others (Pfeffer & Fong, 2005). This propensity influences followers’ desire to enhance their self-image (Yun, Takeuchi, & Liu, 2007) and to discount contrary information (Kunda, 1987). With a natural tendency of followers wanting to associate with “winners,” self-enhancing leaders attract allies, followers, and supporters. Many leaders have access to resources, which enable them to act at will and believe they will not encounter interference or serious consequences from organizational processes that might limit behaviors that are headed in destructive directions (Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). In summary, leaders may venture down paths of unethical and destructive behaviors as a result of formidable self-enhancement motives, which may override other personal values and professional and social expectancies, thereby fostering unethical and destructive behavior.

2.1.2. Escalation of Commitment

A primary reason that leaders may doggedly persist in unethical and/or destructive behaviors, even if these approaches are not producing anticipated results, is a phenomenon described as escalation of commitment. Identified as “escalation” Staw (1976, 1981) and “entrapment” (Rubin & Brockner, 1975), the term describes the decision to continue a course of action in the face of negative feedback about prior resource allocation, uncertainty surrounding the likelihood of goal attainment, and choice about whether to continue (Brockner 1992). Street, Robertson, and Geiger (1997) argued that exposure to escalation situations increases the likelihood that individuals will select unethical decision-making options. Furthermore, leaders may escalate commitment to failing courses of action to (a) align actions with their belief system (Biyalogorsky, Boulding, & Staelin, 2006), (b) avoid cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957), (c) justify or rationalize their behavior (Bobocel & Meyer, 1994; Brockner, 1992), (d) diffuse responsibility and assign blame to others (Whyte, 1991), and (e) heighten prospects of success (Whyte, 1986).

Paradoxically, Whyte, Sakes, and Hook (1997) found a significant positive relationship between a leader’s self-efficacy and the tendency to persist in a failing initiative. They concluded, “[T]hose with a high level of skill and a history of selecting courses of action and making them pay off, are most likely to engage in the pursuit of a failing policy” (p. 428). Keil, Depledge, and Rai (2007) found that problem recognition and escalation of commitment were inversely related and that selective perception and illusion of control significantly affected both. Selective perception describes how people cognitively structure problems based on personal experience. Previous experiences may bias the information sought by leaders and create a tendency to disregard contradictory information (Hogarth, 1987). The illusion of control is the tendency to place success probability above objective reality, such as in the case of those with a previous success history or with entrepreneurs (Keil et al., 2007).

When leaders confront problems, the way forward may be unclear. In escalating commitment to a failing course of action, they may engage in unethical behaviors to counter negative feedback from the environment (McCain, 1986). Street and Street (2006) found that the greater the magnitude of the escalation situation (i.e., how large and broad an impact an escalating situation was in terms of increasing monetary values), the greater the likelihood of unethical behavior. Finally, by virtue of their roles, leaders have positional power, defined as the “capacity to alter others’ states by providing or withholding resources and administering punishments” (Keltner et al., 2003, p. 267). If leaders feel a reduced sense of power, they may escalate their commitment to prevent perceived failure, off-set an impending negative self-portrayal, or reframe their strategies in pursuit of a course of action to minimize negative effects of perceived failure (Raven, 2001). Therefore, escalation of commitment may contribute to the pursuit of unethical and destructive leadership behavior because leaders may persevere in pursuing goals at any and all costs.

2.1.3. Self-Deception

Bronner (2003) enumerated several decision-making pathologies that may explain leaders’ unwillingness to challenge unethical or destructive courses of actions. The pathologies focus on such self-deceptive practices as unrealistic framing of decisions, including the information received about decisions and their related consequences. Self-deception, the paradoxical capacity to deceive oneself regarding the truth in the process of protecting self-image, is a powerful psychological determinant (Tenbrunsel & Messick, 2004). Self-deceivers deny and/or conceal the truth, avoid personal commitment, and display evasive mechanisms including willful ignorance, systematic ignoring, emotional detachment, self-pretense, and rationalization (Fingarette, 2000; Haight, 1980; Martin, 1986).

If leaders fail to seek adequate information, devalue the information sources, and selectively perceive information received, they may cognitively push the moral implications of a decision to the background, allowing them to pursue deviant behaviors, whether consciously or unconsciously (Ludwig & Longenecker, 1993; Tenbrunsel & Messick, 2004). Ironically by ignoring or avoiding moral implications of decisions, self-interested leaders actually can retain self-perceptions that they are ethical leaders. Tenbrusel and Messick described this inductive self-deception process as “ethical numbing,” where repeated exposure to ethical dilemmas fosters tolerance for and support of unethical decision-making, removing leaders from ethical scrutiny, and further reinforcing self-deception. Self-deception of leaders, when combined with positional power and personal persuasion, may then lead to the persistence of unethical and destructive practices.

Proposition 3. Leader characteristics such as personal values, self-enhancement motives, self-deception, and tendencies to escalate commitment will affect leader behavior within organizational processes and perpetrate patterns of unethical and destructive leader behavior.

2.2. Organizational Characteristics and Processes

To further address the question as to why leaders persist in unethical and destructive behaviors, three specific organizational dimensions are addressed: ethical climate and norms, environmental uncertainty, and reactions to perceived threats (threat rigidity).

2.2.1. Ethical Climate and Norms

Ethical climate is influenced by organizational history, structures, and practices including the founder’s and subsequent leaders’ personal values, ethical policies and procedures, legal oversight, decision-making processes, and accountability structures (Dickson, Smith, Grojean, & Ehrhart, 2001; Schein, 1992; Victor & Cullen, 1988). However, the existence of stated ethical organizational policies, such as a code of ethics, does not necessarily inhibit unethical behavior. For example, Webley and Werner (2008) found gaps in organizations’ ethics policies and ethical practices. Such gaps may arise if ethics policies are not reflected in strategic goals and operating methods (Webley, 2003). When top organizational leaders do not regularly espouse and demonstrate behaviors consistent with ethical principles, it is likely that lower level managers and leaders may see ethics codes and policies merely as symbolic, but not necessarily normative, which encourages further deterioration of ethical standards.

Consistent with this assertion, Treviño, Weaver, Gibson, and Toffler (1999) surveyed 10,000 employees in six large U.S. corporations, resulting in three relevant findings. First, if employees were aware of relevant ethical and legal issues, they may be more likely to ask the right questions and ultimately do the right thing when faced with an ethical dilemma. An effective ethics/compliance management program encourages employees to look for ethical/legal advice within the company. Second, in cases where employees perceived that an ethics/compliance program was oriented primarily to protecting top management from blame, employees expressed greater tendencies to undertake unethical behaviors, were less committed to the organization, and rarely reported ethical/legal violations to management. Third, top leadership was a key in creating an effective ethical culture where employees perceived that executives take ethics seriously and consider ethics as important as the bottom line. When workers perceived such a climate was present, they were more aware of ethical issues, were less likely to consider unethical behaviors, and more likely to report unethical conduct.

Dickson et al. (2001) proposed that ethical climate proceeds directly from the values and motives of the organizational founders and early organizational leaders. They argued that ethical climate is created

when organizational founders and subsequent leaders explicitly attend to their own values, determine the values that they wish to instill in organizational members, and make strategic organizational decisions that will facilitate the development of shared perceptions of appropriate ethical behavior. (p. 198)

When environmental conditions are stable and predictable, organizational members generally have been found to follow ethical standards (Dickson et al., 2001). Conversely when the environment is unstable and unpredictable, members confront unfamiliar circumstances, where no precedent informs ethical decision-making. In this case, if ethical values and standards are not well established, leaders’ unethical/destructive behavior becomes more likely. Clear accountability structures are one safeguard against leaders’ unethical conduct.

Accountability within organizations is established when managers and employees expect that they will be called upon to explain, justify, and defend their behaviors and self-evaluations to one or more others (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999, 2003). In support of this assertion, Sedikides, Herbst, Hardin, and Dardis (2002) and Sedikides and Herbst (2002) found that accountability deters self-enhancement because of evaluation expectancy within organizational/social contexts. Although accountability may assist to safeguard ethical decision making, it is not fool-proof, as the effects of accountability are susceptible to dynamics of given situational contexts (Tetlock, 1992). Sedikides et al. (2002) concluded that self-enhancement is a controllable bias in response to admonitions for truthfulness, which accountability is designed to foster.

2.2.2. Environmental Uncertainty

Scholars pose various external factors related to the environment in explaining unethical/destructive leadership and corporate corruption. For example, Baucus (1994) identified both pressure, or the urgent corporate demands or constraints upon a firm; and the need, or the lack of adequate resources, as impacting the tendency to “cut corners” within uncertain environments (p. 704). A scarcity of financial and human resources creates what Pfeffer and Salancik (1978) termed resource dependence, whereby firms depend on stakeholders (i.e., customers, suppliers, regulators, the media) to acquire the needed resources to ensure viability. As a result of inadequate resources, leaders may compromise ethical standards to retain a competitive edge. For example, Staw and Szwajkowski (1975) assessed 87 Fortune 500 firms that had violated Federal Trade Commission Laws and found that firms within environments with scarce resources were more likely to violate the law to cope with external demands than those that were not. Similarly many scholars argue that the greater the stakeholder pressures, the more likely a firm will demonstrate corrupt behavior (DeCelles & Pfarrer, 2004; Finney & Lisieur, 1982; Szwajkowski, 1985). Within a culture of competition and environmental uncertainty (Coleman, 1987), unachievable target goals may further compel leaders to compromise ethical standards (DeCelles & Pfarrer, 2004; Kulik, O’Fallon, & Salimath, 2008).

In addition to stakeholder pressures and resource dependence, environmental complexity coalesces with these and other external factors to create further uncertainty and possible unethical and destructive leadership behavior. Fleming and Zyglidopoulous (2008) proposed a model of organizational deception identifying organizational complexity as a moderating variable in what Elliot and Schroth (2002) termed the “fog of complexity,” where unethical and destructive behavior is “difficult to control or understand as more activities are conducted out of the boundaries of normal managerial control” (p. 104). Because of environmental complexity, the default systems of leaders may amplify their own skewed cognitive filters and blind spots (McNamera, Luce, & Tompson, 2002) and exacerbate ethical compromise. In summary, competition within uncertain environments highlights the organizational demands that turbulence and change processes incur (Palmer & Wiseman, 1999), which may heighten predictable unethical corporate behavior from a loosening of checks and balances to a lack of regulation (Baucus & Near, 1991 ).

2.2.3. Threat-Rigidity Response

The threat-rigidity response (Staw et al., 1981) may help explain why leaders choose to persist in unethical and destructive behaviors. A “threat” is typically defined as a negative or adverse situation over which decision makers have little control and in which there is a potential for salient loss (Chattopadhyay, Glick, & Huber, 2001; Dutton & Jackson, 1987). Given competition amid times of environmental uncertainty, turbulence, scarcity of resources, and change, leaders are exposed to conditions outside their control and repertoire that threatens to impede their work. Therefore, they may seek to reduce uncertainty by resorting to behaviors characterized by caution, restriction in information processing, and centralization of control (Chattopadhyay et al., 2001; Harrington, Lemak, & Kendall, 2002; Sitkin & Pablo, 1992). In so doing, leaders may centralize their power, resulting in further rigidity. Staw et al. (1981) argued that threat-rigidity effects, which create psychological stress and anxiety, can either be healthy and functional or maladaptive and dysfunctional. If maladaptive, leaders may respond to threats by engaging in unethical and destructive behaviors to reduce the perceived threats from the environment for self-protection or organizational survival and viability. As a result, legitimate threats to the organization may lead to intergroup conflict, competition for resources, and pressures for uniformity, which augment leadership control and the restriction of information, further enhancing unethical tendencies. Conversely, toxic leaders may use perceived threats for personal advantage as a way to heighten follower dependency and exercise inordinate control (Kellerman, 2004; Lipman-Blumen, 2005).

A different theoretical lens, “prospect theory,” has also been applied to threat situations proffering different conclusions. According to prospect theory, people who perceive themselves to be in a negative situation tend to seek risk to better their position (Case & Shane, 1998; Kahneman & Lavallo, 1993; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). While prospect theory may appear at first to contradict the threat-rigidity thesis, Chattopadhyay et al. (2001) suggested that the risk-seeking response of prospect theory will occur when a threat is perceived as controllable, while the threat-rigidity response will be manifested when a threat is perceived as uncontrollable. Palmer, Danforth, and Clark (1995) found evidence for both prospect theory and threat-rigidity response in a study of hospitals. Poor performing hospitals exhibited some riskier courses of action than superior performing hospitals. However, these risk-taking actions seemed to be embedded in an overall rigid response pattern and may have been viewed within the threatened organizations as a single risky alternative pursued within a general frame of control.

Because the threat-rigidity response is characterized by restricting some types of information flow, increasing emphasis on efficiency, and centralizing decision-making (D’Aveni, 1989), it tends to limit the capability of the organization to adapt to changing conditions and to increase the focus of top management on short-term objectives over longer-term goals. Likewise, perceived resource shortages may lead to reduction of the number of managers, leading to greater concentration of authority and power in the remaining managers. In these cases, perceptions and reactions to perceived organizational threats may prompt leaders’ unethical and destructive behavior to protect self-esteem through a self-serving bias (Campbell & Sedikides, 1999) and/or to insure organizational viability at all costs (Chattopadhyay et al., 2001).

Proposition 4. Organizational conditions such as ethical climate, norms, and accountability; environmental uncertainty; and perceived threats to organizational survival will impact organizational processes and reduce limitations on destructive behavior of leaders.

3. WHY ARE UNETHICAL AND/OR DESTRUCTIVE LEADERSHIP BEHAVIORS TOLERATED BY FOLLOWERS AND OTHER ORGANIZATIONAL STAKEHOLDERS?

Although leaders may acknowledge the benefits of ethical leadership reflecting values of corporate social responsibility, honesty, integrity, and believing that “ethics pay” (Rhode, 2006), many violate the trust of stakeholders, not the least of whom are followers, when engaging in unethical and/or destructive leadership behavior. Leader behaviors impacting followers include greed, lying, cheating, intentional deception, theft, extortion, and interpersonal manipulation, to name a few (Bok, 1999; Cruver, 2002; Doris, 2002; Martin, 1986). This section addresses specific follower and organizational characteristics which contribute to followers and stake holders tolerating unethical and potentially destructive leader behaviors.

3.1. Follower Characteristics

In addition to leader and organizational characteristics and processes which impact unethical and destructive leadership behavior, followers likewise have been shown to exert influence on leader behavior (Berg, 1998; Kellerman, 2008; Offerman, 2004; Rost, 1993). As Whicker (1996) argued: “To blame the decline of many institutions and organizations in the United States on bad leadership is to oversimplify the complex relationship between leaders and followers” (p. 51). Followers may play a highly instrumental role in supporting the unethical behavior of leaders by passive or active complicity (Kellerman, 2004; Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007; Vardi & Weitz, 2004). Six specific follower characteristics contribute to the active and/or passive support of the leaders’ unethical/destructive behavior. The most salient characteristics related to followers and other organizational stakeholders are (a) self-concept, (b) self-efficacy and LOC, (c) values, (d) role modeling, (e) social identity, and (f) organizational commitment.

3.1.1. Self-Concept

Follower self-concept relates to the ways followers view themselves and their self-worth. Thought to derive from individuals’ knowledge of themselves including personality, image of one’s physical appearance, persona, and self-schemas, Lord and Brown (2004) defined the self-concept as “the overarching knowledge structure that organizes memory and behavior … and includes trait-like schemas that organize social and self-perceptions in specific situations” (p. 14). They argued that leaders’ behaviors are proximal determinants of followers’ self-concept activation. Contributing to the development of the followers’ self-concept, the leader-member exchange (LME) process fosters a psychological interaction enabling followers to experience protection and security, achievement and effectiveness, inclusion and belongingness, and commitment and loyalty (Messick, 2005). Graen and Uhl-Bien (1995) described the outcome of this psychological exchange as the formation of in-groups and out-groups, dyadic relationships of contrasting degrees of trust, interaction, and closeness. In-groups comprise the inner circle and can be conformers or colluders in leaders’ unethical behavior (Padilla et al., 2007). As such, Lord, Brown, and Freiberg (1999) cited that research has supported several precursors to (LME), namely leaders “liking” followers, follower demographics, and perceived attitudinal similarity (cf. Engle & Lord, 1997). As a result, followers’ self-concepts may be strengthened leading to further motivation, self-regulation, and information processes. Since people are motivated to preserve and increase their sense of self-esteem and status, followers will be highly motivated to preserve their self-identity, especially in their relationship to leaders within social contexts (Hogg, 2001; Hogg & Reid, 2001; Shamir, House, & Arthur, 1993).

The self-concept may be reinforced by the roles followers or stakeholders play in relationship to leaders (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). For example, leaders may increase followers’ self-esteem through appraisals, performance evaluations, and other positive reinforcements and rewards. If leaders are engaging in the slippery slope of unethical behavior, followers may unwittingly contribute to the process by remaining silent, unwilling to confront superiors about unethical behavior (Kellerman, 2004). If as Berg (1998) suggested the responsibility for the leader-follower relationship resides with the follower and if the underlying leader expectation is an “unchanging request for obedience,” then the likelihood of unethical and destructive behaviors being supported by followers will be strengthened (p. 33).

3.1.5. Social Identity

When followers attract to various organizational settings and roles through the ASA cycle (Schneider, 1987), they engage in a socialization process whereby shared beliefs, attitudes, expectations, and behaviors are interwoven with leaders, inclusive of the organizational vision and culture (Hogg, 2005). In-group association and distinctiveness, based on basic positive self-concepts needs, coalesce into in-group behaviors and prototypes. When followers are socially adept, they may identify more strongly with the organization and desire favored treatment by leaders. In the psychological exchange between followers and leaders, followers glean a sense of vision and direction, protection and security, inclusion and belonging, personal effectiveness, and pride, self-respect, and direction (Messick, 2005). In exchange, followers reciprocate by offering gratitude, loyalty, commitment, sacrifice, and obedience. Therefore when followers and leaders like each other, valuable LMEs benefit both parties (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). As Lord and Brown (2001) observed, since followers desire to nurture positive self-evaluations based on these exchanges, they may be “especially sensitive to the affective feedback from leaders, using it as a basis for constructing a reflected self-identity” (p. 42). Leaders also activate and reinforce follower identity in the modeling of values as they relate to goal achievement (Lord & Brown, 2001), resulting in powerful psychological and emotional attachments (Popper, 2001). Because of this beneficial LME, followers may heighten organizational commitment and, in so doing, may acquiesce to unethical leadership behaviors to maintain their social standing, relational connections, and attachment to the leader (Brass et al., 1998; O’Reilly & Chatman, 1986).

3.1.6. Organizational Commitment

Organizational commitment of followers has been the subject of research related to unethical behavior (Cullinan, Bline, Farrar, & Lowe, 2008; Peterson, 2003; Street, 1995) and is distinctive from escalation of commitment. Organizational commitment is a multidimensional construct involving affective attachment to and identification with the organization (Meyer & Allen, 1991; Mowday, Porter, & Steers, 1982; Porter, Steers, Mowday, & Boulian, 1974). If as O’Reilly and Chatman (1986) argued, that organizational commitment is predicated on followers’ psychological attachment and identification with the goals, values, and characteristic of the organization, then we maintain that followers with high levels of organizational commitment may experience cognitive dissonance in the face of leaders’ unethical behavior. In order to uphold socially constructed organizational identity, followers therefore may deny problems to avoid the negative intrapersonal residue that the awareness of ethical/destructive behavior brings. In such cases, followers may ignore the leadership infraction, rationalize it away, or react to it through moral agency. Examples include Enron employees who realized something was awry but because of the company reputation were compliant and complacent in the face of tell-tale signs of leadership malfeasance (Cruver, 2002).

Various antecedents impact organizational commitment including positive factors such as job satisfaction, job security, and employee performance, as well as negative factors including role ambiguity, role conflict, incongruent job-fit alignment, and organizational turnover (Porter et al., 1974). Research examining the relationship between organizational commitment and ethical decision making reveal that followers with higher organizational commitment were less likely to engage in unethical behavior leading to personal gain and harmful organizational outcomes (Tang & Chiu, 2003). Cullinan et al.’s (2008) study involving employees from three companies found similar results, suggesting that followers with higher levels of organizational commitment may have a longer-term orientation, which would overshadow consideration of the short-term personal gains derived from unethical behavior.

When organizations face decline and crisis, five behavioral modes of commitment have been identified: (a) exit, active negative commitment, (b) voice, active positive commitment, (c) loyalty, passive positive commitment, (d) neglect, positive negative commitment, and (e) silence (Bar-Haim, 2007; Hirshman, 1970; Rusbult, Farrell, Rogers, & Mainous, 1988). We suggest that in the face of unethical leadership behavior followers with high levels of organizational commitment, LOC, and a strong belief/value system will be more apt to remain loyal to the organization, retain a sense of voice, and resist unethical/destructive leadership behavior. Or if the pressures to engage in unethical behaviors persist, followers’ commitment may decrease and their intention to leave will escalate (Peterson, 2003). Conversely, those who do not have high levels of organizational commitment will be more inclined to make an exit (active negative commitment), become neglectful of their jobs (positive negative commitment), or be more obliging in allowing unethical leadership behavior to continue (silence).

In support of this assertion, Street (1995) predicted that the high levels of organizational commitment and cognitive moral development of followers would increase the likelihood of whistle-blowing. The organizational commitment of followers is an important variable in answering the question related to why leaders persist in behaviors that they knew were unethical and destructive. Factors influencing followers’ passive acceptance of, collusion with, or active position against unethical behavior may be impacted by their tenure with the organization and the size of the organization (cf. Dozier & Miceli, 1985). In summary, followers’ self-concepts, self-efficacy and LOC, values, role modeling behavior, social identity, and organizational commitment interact with leader characteristics and organizational processes in contributing to the support of leaders’ unethical and destructive behavior.

Proposition 5. Characteristics of followers including self-concept, self-efficacy, LOC, value alignment, need for role modeling, social identity, and organizational commitment will combine to inhibit follower willingness to engage organizational processes that would limit destructive leader behaviors.

3.2. Organizational Dynamics

The reason followers and other organizational stakeholders tolerate unethical and destructive leadership behavior relates specifically to conducive environments. Factors previously identified as important antecedents include environmental instability, threat perception, cultural values, lack of checks and balances, and effective structural accountability (Padilla et al., 2007; Sedikides et al., 2002). Our focus includes three additional organizational processes: evaluations of actions, the feedback environment, and conformity dynamics.

3.2.2. Feedback Environment

The feedback environment refers to the contextual aspects of day-to-day supervisor-subordinate and coworker-coworker feedback processes, as opposed to the formal performance appraisal feedback (Steelman, Levy, & Snell, 2004). Feedback within organizational contexts has informational value, which assist leaders and followers meet goals and regulate behavior (Ashford, Blatt, & VandeWalle, 2003; VandeWalle, 2003). The feedback environment may suggest to followers, peers, and other stakeholders the degree of organizational support for feedback to leaders about their behavior. If the feedback environment discourages inquiry related to job performance and personal interaction, we contend that unethical and destructive leadership behavior will be more likely. Key determinants of feedback-seeking behavior relate to the perception of the ego cost of doing so (Ashford & Cummings, 1983), as influenced by goal orientation (Ashford et al., 2003).

For example, Whitaker, Dahling, and Levy (2007) posited that individuals who focused more on learning, as opposed to performance outcomes, were less concerned about the ego cost of seeking feedback. This supports the notion that high power and performance-oriented leaders would likely resist feedback seeking behavior, which could reinforce power disinhibition and unethical behavior. If, as Whitaker et al. suggested, feedback-seeking behavior leads to role clarity, then job performance will likely be a positive outcome. However as Ashford et al. (2003) cautioned, the motives and outcomes for seeking or quenching feedback seeking behavior should be evaluated (i.e., to perform well, defend/enhance leader ego, or enhance impression management), as the cost of isolation from feedback may incur devastating results, as seen in the recent avalanche of corporate scandals.

In a study of 150 subordinate–supervisor dyads across a variety of organizations, Rosen, Levy, and Hall (2006) found that when employees have greater access to information regarding behaviors that are acceptable and desired at work, perceptions of politics are reduced, and work outcomes are enhanced. Feedback environments supporting high levels of informal supervisor and coworker interaction are also associated with higher employee morale, as indicated by job satisfaction and organizational commitment, resulting in higher levels of organizational citizenship. Both positive and negative feedback are both essential for organizational viability, especially as leaders model the feedback-seeking process. As Herold and Parsons (1985) asserted, “negative feedback is not only important but needs to be assessed independently of positive feedback” (p. 304). Thus, we maintain that organizational environments typified by feedback-seeking on all organizational levels will increase job performance and lesson the propensity for unethical and destructive leadership behavior.

3.2.3. Conformity Pressures

Research on situational factors impacting human behavior has revealed the power of context in shaping how people respond in given situations (Bandura, 1999). Conformity in the perpetration of unethical and destructive behavior, particularly as it relates to leaders and followers in specific contexts, has received research attention. In particular, three research initiatives demonstrate the process of de-individuation, dehumanization, and the evil of inaction among participants who were ordinary people. The outcome of these studies, as summarized below, affirms the powerful effect of conformity processes when faced with group pressures to acquiesce to a certain view point or with pressures from authority figures to engage in unethical and/or destructive behavior.

One classic study on conformity to group norms undertaken by social psychologist Solomon Asch (1951, 1955) related to visual perception. Asch predicted that few participants would conform to the majority’s obvious incorrect responses related to judging the relative length of lines written on cards. When making solo decisions, participants made correct judgments 99% of the time. However when participants answered as a part of a group, after respondents intentionally gave incorrect answers, they responded incorrectly about 75% of the time. Only 25% of the participants were able to maintain their correct answers, despite the group’s differing opinion.

In a second social conformity study known as the 1971 Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE), Zimbardo (2006, 2007; Zimbardo, Maslach, & Haney, 1999) observed that situational context had a powerful effect on the behaviors of students placed in the roles of guards and prisoners in a simulated prison environment. Designed to assess the effects of social distance and pressures for conformity within emerging group norms, some “guards” came to abuse their power by demeaning and degrading the “prisoners,” actually fellow students; while others either went through their jobs methodically or were more reluctant to adopt more abusive approaches. However, none of the “good” guards intervened to limit the abusive behaviors of other guards, nor actively complained either to their abusive peers or to the supervisors of the simulation. The psychological and emotional experimental effects on participants were so detrimental that the 14-day experiment was abruptly called off after 6 days.

Zimbardo’s (2007) results are of particular interest when considering how pressures for conformity may stifle follower or peer tendencies to speak out against unethical or destructive leadership behaviors. When people are placed in relatively unusual settings where the rules for behaviors and interactions may be ill-defined and not perceived as consistent with previous experiences, a set of dynamic psychological processes may induce people to do evil, which Zimbardo termed “the Lucifer effect.” The behaviors adopted by these SPE “emergent leaders” may have provided a kind of sense-giving for other members of the group taking up relatively unfamiliar roles. Indeed, sense-giving, in which information is provided to help organizational members understand themselves, the nature of their work roles, and their relationships with others in the organization is a critical role for organizational leaders (Foldy, Goldman, & Ospina, 2008). However, this sense-giving gone awry by students assigned as guards illustrates the relative power of situations to incrementally introduce ‘evil’ into the average behaviors of otherwise good people. In more typical organizational situations, it may be similarly easy for persons working without contact with individual customers, constituents, or students to become cognitively caught up in a similar “us/them” perceptual trap. In such a case, Zimbardo’s work (1969, 2006, 2007) illustrates the relative ease with which peers and followers alike can become compliant with the expectations and pressures to conform and accept unethical and/ or destructive behaviors of leaders.

A third conformity study conducted by Milgram’s (1974) on obedience to authority likewise demonstrated that average people will conform to authority even when it could possibly bring harm to others. Asked to apply electric shock as punishment in the process of assisting people improve their memory in the learning process, over two-thirds (67%) of all participants (i.e., teachers) actually applied the highest shock levels of 450 volts at the urging of the authority figure supervising the setting, despite the desperate (but simulated) cries of the learners. The results convincingly demonstrated that one situational variable could cause compliance rates of over 90% of participants and conversely drop to a low of 10%. Situational context proved to be a powerful determiner of behavior, which in this simulated obedience study, surprised even Milgram himself.

All three of these studies affirm that, given specific situational contexts, average people may conform to dysfunctional group norms and disengage morally in supporting destructive conduct when authority figures so dictate and/or they justify their own behavior and minimize personal responsibility (Bandura, 1999; Bandura et al., 1996).

Proposition 6. Organizational characteristics such as prevailing evaluation of actions, the feedback environment, and pressures for conformity will combine to reduce the occurrence and effects of organizational processes designed to limit destructive leader behaviors.

SUMMARY

We set out in this chapter to consider how characteristics of leaders, followers, and organizational settings combine with organizational processes to provide answers to three questions. The questions were: (a) how and why do leaders venture down unethical and destructive paths?, (b) why do leaders persist in unethical and destructive behaviors?, and (c) why are unethical and/or destructive leadership behaviors tolerated by followers and other organizational stakeholders? After considering alternative perspectives related to each question, we believe we have demonstrated that considering the processes within organizations provides a powerful approach for understanding how destructive leadership arises and persists in organizations. Specifically, better understanding of destructive leadership requires consideration of the organizational processes that place people in leadership roles and limit internal regulation, which subsequently enable leaders to continue down paths that are detrimental to the organization and its members. Focusing on the processes that are the essence of how organizations operate enabled us to provide additional perspectives in which to evaluate how and why unethical and destructive leadership behaviors occur and explain why organizational members ignore the obvious signposts these behaviors present.

Within the unique dynamics between leaders, followers, and organizational/situational contexts, the complexity and potential fallout of unethical and destructive leadership resulting in damaged lives and organizational demise cannot be understated. Building upon previous conceptual frameworks (Brown & Treviño, 2006a; Jones, 1991; Padilla et al., 2007), we offered new lenses for viewing unethical and destructive leadership by exploring additional social-cognitive and management processes applied to unethical and destructive leadership, such as the leadership selection process, leaders’ escalation of commitment, self-enhancement motives, followers’ organizational commitment, and the organizational processes impacted by goal setting pressures, threat rigidity, the feedback environment, and conformity dynamics.

Our analyses suggest that leader characteristics as well as internal and external variables may impact organizational processes limiting the ability of these processes to regulate and safeguard against unethical and destructive leader behaviors. From a practical viewpoint, increased awareness of these possible limitations may help organizations develop and implement watchful approaches to identify and curb destructive and/or unethical leadership behaviors at an early stage. Clearly the more prepared an organization may be to identify and curb such behaviors, the more likely it is that norms for regulations within the organization will be reinforced and have the desired effect.

Directions for Future Research

The propositions presented above provide the primary basis for future research concerning how the interplay of leader, follower, and organizational characteristics within organizational processes may unintentionally facilitate destructive leadership. These propositions are summarized in Table 5.1.

We anticipate that testing proposition 1 may have significant payoff both theoretically and practically, in that the underlying implicit leadership models involved in leadership selection decisions are rarely made known. Consequently, the extent to which such characteristics as cognitive moral development and responsibility dispositions of leadership candidates are considered important attributes and thus are thoroughly investigated in the selection process is also unclear. In addition, impression management efforts may be more successful for candidates with personality tendencies that include narcissism. Better understanding of the evaluation criteria used by participants in the selection process will be critical for limiting the acceptance of persons who arrive in leadership roles with tendencies that increase the likelihood of unethical and destructive behaviors.

Table 5.1. Propositions for Future Research

Proposition 1. Leader selection processes are likely to overlook underlying leader characteristics such as LOC, narcissism, level of moral development, and responsibility disposition which may affect leader actions in organizational processes and limit the effects of regulatory safeguards to curb destructive leadership behaviors.
Proposition 2. Organizational characteristics such as the prevailing ethical climate, pressure to set aggressive goals, and intense competition will combine to limit the impact that organizational processes may have on regulating destructive leader behaviors.
Proposition 3. Leader characteristics such as personal values, self-enhancement motives, self-deception, and tendencies to escalate commitment will affect leader behavior within organizational processes and perpetrate patterns of destructive leader behaviors.
Proposition 4. Organizational conditions such as ethical climate, norms, and accountability; environmental uncertainty; and perceived threats to organizational survival will impact organizational processes and reduce limitations on destructive leader behaviors.
Proposition 5. Characteristics of followers including self-concept, self-efficacy, LOC, value alignment, need for role modeling, social identity, and organizational commitment will combine to inhibit follower willingness to engage organizational processes that would limit destructive leader behaviors.
Proposition 6. Organizational characteristics such as prevailing explanations for actions, the feedback environment, and pressures for conformity will combine to reduce the occurrence and effects of organizational processes designed to limit destructive leader behaviors.

Examination of the additional five propositions will largely bring to light the effects of both individual and organizational level variables in establishing and implementing norms that serve to regulate leader behaviors and bring leaders back from destructive behavioral patterns. We particularly anticipate that better understanding of the interplay between organizational pressures and the individual characteristics of leaders holds substantial promise. For example, self-enhancement motives of leaders may have limited negative effects until the leader’s organization or unit is faced with threats to survival. When faced with perceived threats, these leaders may have increased tendencies toward rigidity in behaviors, limiting information flow and blocking scrutiny of their behaviors.

Besides these propositions, our analyses suggest additional questions requiring further exploration: (a) what additional leadership selection criteria and assessments need to be developed to accurately evaluate “dark side” leader and follower characteristics to assist organizations prevent unethical/destructive leadership (Hogan et al., 1994)?, (b) in what organizational or situational contexts will leaders of proven integrity bow to unethical/destructive leadership behavior?, (c) what do exemplars of ethical organizational climate teach us about how to avoid unethical practices related to structural and accountability processes?, and (d) how might business and corporate ethics training be radically reevaluated and revised to include the most up-to-date research in real-world contexts to foster applied praxis? Insights from such research would have long-range implications in strengthening leaders, followers, and organizational processes in order to effectively address the obvious signposts of unethical and destructive leadership, foster ethical behavior and sustainability at all organizational levels, and promote public trust.

REFERENCES

American Management Association. (2006). The ethical enterprise: A global study of business ethics (2005–2015). New York: Author.

Anderson, C. R. (1977). Locus of control, coping behaviors, and performance in stress settings: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62(4), 446-451.

Anderson, L. M., & Bateman, T. S. (1997). Cynicism in the workplace: Some causes and effects. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18(5), 449-469.

Asch, S. E. (1951). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70, Whole No 416.

Asch, S. E. (1955, Nov.). Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American, 193, 31-35.

Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1983). Feedback as an individual resource: Personal strategies of creating information. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 32, 370-398.

Ashford, S. J., Blatt, R., & VandeWalle, D. (2003). Reflections on the looking glass: A review of research on feedback-seeking behavior in organizations. Journal of Management, 29(6), 773-799.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(3), 193-209.

Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (2), 364-374.

Bar-Haim, A. (2007). Rethinking organizational commitment in relation to perceived organizational power and perceived employment alternatives. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 7(2), 203-217.

Barsky, A. (2008). Understanding the ethical cost of organizational goal-setting: A review and theory development. Journal of Business Ethics, 81(1), 63-81.

Bass, B. M., & Steidlmeier, P. (1999). Ethics, character, and authentic transformational leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 10(2), 181-217.

Baucus, M. S. (1994). Pressure, opportunity and predisposition: A multivariate model of corporate illegality. Journal of Management, 20(4), 699-721.

Baucus, M. S., & Near, J. P. (1991). Can illegal corporate behavior be predicted? An event history analysis. Academy of Management Journal, 34(1), 9-36.

Bazerman, M. H., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). The social psychology of ordinary unethical behavior. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 111-115.

Beauregard, K. S., & Dunning D. (1998). Turning up the contrast: Self-enhancement motives prompt egocentric contrast effects in social judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 606-621.

Benson, M. J., & Hogan, R. (2008). How dark side leadership personality destroys trust and degrades organizational effectiveness. Organizations & People, 15(3), 10-19.

Berg, D. N. (1998). Resurrecting the muse: Followership in organizations. In E. B. Klein, F. Gabelnick, & P. Herr (Eds.), The psychodynamics of leadership (pp. 27-52). Madison, CT: Psychosocial Press.

Biyalogorsky, E., Boulding, W., & Staelin, R. (2006). Stuck in the past: Why managers persist with new product failures. Journal of Marketing, 70(2), 108-121.

Bobocel, D. R., & Meyer, J. P. (1994). Escalating commitment to a failing course of action: Separating the roles of choice and justification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(3), 360-363.

Bok, S. (1999). Lying: Moral choice in public and private life. New York: Vintage Books.

Brass, D. J., Butterfield, K. D., & Scaggs, B. C. (1998). Relationships and unethical behavior: A social network perspective. Academy of Management Review, 23(1), 14-31.

Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (1), 83-93.

Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing course of action. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 39.

Bronner, R. (2003). Pathologies of decision-making: Causes, forms, and handling. Management International Review, 43(1), 85-101.

Brown, M. E., & Treviño, L. K. (2006a). Ethical leadership: A review and future directions. The Leadership Quarterly, 17(6), 595-616.

Brown, M. E., & Treviño, L. K. (2006b, August). Role modeling and ethical leadership. Paper presented at the 2006 Academy of Management Annual Meeting. Atlanta, GA.

Cameron, K. (2003). Ethics, virtuousness, and constant change. In N. M. Tichy & A. R. McGill (Eds.), The ethical challenge: How to lead with unyielding integrity (pp. 185-194). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 3(1), 23-43.

Case, R., & Shane, S. (1998). Fostering risk taking in research and development: The importance of a project’s terminal value. Decision Sciences, 29(4), 765-783.

Chattopadhyay, P., Glick, W., & Huber, G. (2001). Organizational actions in response to threats and opportunities. Academy of Management Journal, 44(5), 937-955.

Coleman, J. W. (1987). Toward an integrated theory of white-collar crime. American Journal of Sociology, 93(2), 406-439.

Collins, J. (2001). Good to Great. New York: HarperCollins.

Cooper, W. H. (1981). Ubiquitous halo. Psychological Bulletin, 90(2), 218-244.

Cruver, B. (2002). Anatomy of greed: The un-shredded truth from an Enron insider. New York: Carroll & Graf.

Cullen, J. B., Parboteeach, K. P., & Victor, B. (2003). The effects of ethical climate on organizational commitment: Two-study analysis. Journal of Business Ethics, 46(2), 127-141.

Cullinan, C., Bline, D., Farrar, R., & Lowe, D. (2008). Organization-harm vs. organization-gain ethical issues: An exploratory examination of the effects of organizational commitment. Journal of Business Ethics, 80(2), 225-235.

D’Aveni, R. A. (1989). The aftermath of organizational decline: A longitudinal study of the strategic and managerial characteristics of declining firms. Academy of Management Journal, 32(3), 577-605.

DeCelles, K. A., & Pfarrer, M. D. (2004). Heroes or villains? Corruption and the charismatic leader. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 1 1 (1), 67 -7 7.

De Cremer, D., & van Dijk, E. (2008). Leader-follower effects in resource dilemmas : The roles of leadership selection and social responsibility. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11 (3), 355-369.

De Hoogh, A. H. B., & Den Hartog, D. N. (2008). Ethical and despotic leadership, relationships with leader’s social responsibility, top management team effectiveness and subordinates’ optimism: A multi-method study. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(3), 297-311.

DeVries, D. L. (1992). Executive selection: Advances but no progress. Issues & Observations, 12(4), 1-5.

Dickson, M. W., Smith, D. B., Grojean, M. W., & Ehrhart, M. (2001). An organizational climate regarding ethics: The outcome of leader values and the practices that reflect them. The Leadership Quarterly, 12(2), 197-217.

Doris, J. M. (2002). Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Dotlich, D. L., & Cairo, P. D. (2003). Why CEOs fail: The 11 behaviors that can derail our climb to the top and how to manage them. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Dozier, J. B., & Miceli, M. P. (1985). Potential predictors of whistle-blowing: A prosocial behavior perspective. Academy of Management Review, 10(4), 823-836.

Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57(6), 1082-1090.

Dutton, J., & Jackson, S. (1987). Categorizing strategic issues: Links to organizational action. Academy of Management Review, 31 (1), 85-105.

Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. S., & Skogstad, A. (2007). Destructive leadership behaviour : A definition and conceptual model. The Leadership Quarterly, 18(3), 207-216.

Elliot, A. L., & Schroth, R. J. (2002). How companies lie : Why Enron is just the tip of the iceberg. London, England: Nicholas Brealey.

Engle, E. M., & Lord, R. G. (1997). Implicit theories, self-schema, and leader-member exchange. Academy of Management Journal, 40(4), 988-1010.

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.

Fingarette, H. (2000). Self-deception (2nd ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Finney, H. C., & Lesieur, H. R. (1982). A contingency theory of organizational crime. In S. B. Bacharach, Research in the sociology of organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 255-299). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: Core motives in social psychology. New York: Wiley.

Fleming, P., & Zyglidopoulous, S. C. (2008). The escalation of deception in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 81 (4), 837-850.

Foldy, E. G., Goldman, L., & Ospina, S. (2008). Sensegiving and the role of cognitive shifts in the work of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(5), 514-529.

Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to leadership: Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain approach. The Leadership Quarterly, 6(2), 219-247.

Grojean, M. W., Resick, C. J., Dickson, M. W., & Smith, D. B. (2004). Leaders, values, and organizational climate: Examining leadership strategies for establishing an organizational climate regarding ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 55(3), 223-241.

Haight, M. R. (1980). A study of self-deception. Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press.

Harrington, R., Lemak, D., & Kendall, K. (2002). The threat-rigidity thesis in newly formed teams: An empirical test. Journal of Business and Management, 8(2), 127-145.

Harshman, C. L., & Harshman, E. F. (2008). The Gordian knot of ethics: Understanding leadership effectiveness and ethical behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 78(1-2), 175-192.

Heath, E. (2002). Morality & the market: Ethics & virtue in the conduct of business. Boston: McGraw Hill.

Herold. C. K., & Parsons, D. M. (1985). Assessing the feedback environment in work organizations: Development of the job feedback survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70(2), 290-305.

Hirshman, A. O. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hogan, R. (2006). Personality and the fate of organizations. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2008). Learning a lesson in executive selection. Issues & Observations, 27(6), 22-24.

Hogan, R., Curphy, G. J., & Hogan, J. (1994). What we know about leadership: Effectiveness and personality. American Psychologist, 49(6), 493-504.

Hogan, R., Raskin, R., & Fazzini, D. (1990). The dark side of charisma. In K. E. Clark & M. B. Clark (Eds.), Measures of leadership (pp. 343-354). West Orange, NJ: Leadership Library of America.

Hogarth, R. M. (1987). Judgment and choice: The psychology of decision (2nd ed.) New York: Wiley.

Hogg, M. A. (2001). Social categorization, depersonalization, and group behavior. In M. A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Group processes (pp. 56-85). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Hogg, M. A. (2005). Social identity and leadership. In D. M. Messick & R. M. Kramer (Ed.), The psychology of leadership: New perspectives and research (pp. 53-80). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hogg, M. A., & Reid, S. A. (2001). Social identity, leadership, and power. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The use and abuse of power: Multiple perspectives on the causes of corruption (pp. 159-180). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

Hoyt, C. L, Simon, S., & Reid, L. (2009). Choosing the best (wo)man for the job: The effects of mortality salience, sex and gender stereotypes on leader evaluations. The Leadership Quarterly, 20(2), 233-246.

Illies, J. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2008). Responding destructively in leadership situations: The role of personal values and problem construction. Journal of Business Ethics, 82(1), 251-272.

Janssen, O., & Prins, J. (2007). Goal orientations and the seeking of different types of feedback information. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80(2), 235-249.

John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 206-219.

Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366-395.

Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Hoshimo-Brown, E., & Correll, J. (2003). Secure and defensive high self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 969-987.

Judge, T., Colbert, A. E., & Ilies, R. (2004) Intelligence and leadership: A quantitative review and test of theoretical propositions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 542-552.

Jung, D. I., & Avolio, B. J. (2000). Opening the black box: An experimental investigation of the mediating effects of trust and value congruence on transformational and transactional leadership. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21 (8), 949-964.

Kahneman, D., & Lavallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: A cognitive perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39(1), 17-31.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263 -291.

Keil, M., Depledge, G., & Rai, A. (2007). Escalation: The role of problem recognition and cognitive bias. Decision Sciences, 38(3), 391-421.

Kellerman, B. (2004). BAD Leadership: What it is, how it happens, why it matters. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Kellerman, B. (2008). How followers are creating change and changing leaders. Boston: Harvard School Press.

Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110(2), 265-284.

Kets de Vries, M. F. R. (1993). Leaders, fools, and impostors: Essays on the psychology of leadership. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Kets de Vries, M. F. R. (2006). The spirit of despotism: Understanding the tyrant within. Human Relations, 59(2), 195-220.

Kets de Vries, M. F. R., & Miller D. (1997). Narcissism and leadership: An object relations perspective. In R. P. Vecchio (Ed.), Leadership: Understanding the dynamics of power and influence in organizations (pp. 194-214). Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-development approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-480). Chicago: Rand McNally.

Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco: Harper & Row.

Kohlberg, L., Levine, C., & Hewer, A. (1983). Moral stages: A current formulation and a response to critics. Basil, Switzerland: S. Karger.

Kohut, H. (1966). Forms and transformations of narcissism. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 14, 243-272.

Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. New York: Basic Books.

Kramer, R. M. (2003, October). The harder they fall. Harvard Business Review, 81 (10), 58-66.

Kulik, B. W., O’Fallon, M. J., & Salimath, M. S. (2008). Do competitive environments lead to the rise and spread of unethical behavior? Parallels from Enron. Journal of Business Ethics, 83(4), 703–723.

Kunda, Z. (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 37-54.

Lefcourt, H. M. (1966). Internal-external control of reinforcement: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 65(4), 206-220.

Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of accountability. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 255-275.

Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (2003). The impact of accountability on cognitive bias: Bridging individual, interpersonal, and institutional approaches to judgment and choice. In S. Schneider & J. Shanteau (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision making (pp. 431-457). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Lewicki, R. J. (1983). Lying and deception: A behavioral model. In M. H. Bazerman & R. J. Lewicki (Eds.), Negotiating in organizations (pp. 68–90). Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE.

Lipman-Blumen, J. (2005). The allure of toxic leaders: Why we follow destructive bosses and corrupt politicians—and how we can survive them. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Littig, L. W., & Sanders, J. A. (1979). Locus of control and persistence: Effects of skill and chance sets on session and post-session indices. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 13, 387-389.

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Lord, R. G., & Brown, D. J. (2001). Leadership, values, and subordinate self-concepts. The Leadership Quarterly, 12(2), 133-152.

Lord, R. G., & Brown, D. J. (2004). Leadership processes and follower self-identity. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Lord, R. G. Brown, D. J., Freiberg, S. J. (1999). Understanding the dynamics of leadership: The role of follower self-concepts in the leader/follower relationship. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78(3), 167-203.

Ludwig, D. C., & Longenecker, C. O. (1993). The Bathsheba syndrome: The ethical failure of successful leaders. Journal of Business Ethics, 12(4), 265-273.

Maccoby, M. (2000). Narcissistic leaders: The incredible pros, the inevitable cons. Harvard Business Review, 78(1), 69-77.

Maccoby, M. (2003). The productive narcissist: The promise and peril of visionary leadership. New York: Broadway Books.

Manz, C. C., Anand, V., Joshi, M., & Manz, K. P. (2008). Emerging paradoxes in executive leadership: A theoretical interpretation of tensions between corruption and virtuous values. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(3), 385-392.

Martin, M. W. (1986). Self-deception and morality. Lawrence, KS: University Press of Kansas.

McCain, B. E. (1986). Continuing investment under conditions of failure: A laboratory study of the limits to escalation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71 (2), 280-284.

McNamara, G., Luce, R. A., & Tompson, G. H. (2002). Examining the effect of complexity in strategic group knowledge structures on firm performance. Strategic Management Journal, 23(2), 153-170.

Meglino, B. M., & Ravlin, E. C. ( 1998).Individual values in organizations: Concepts, controversies, and research. Journal of Management, 24(3), 351-389.

Meindl, J. R., & Ehrlich, S. B. (1987). The Romance of Leadership and the Evaluation of Organizational Performance. Academy of Management Journal, 30(1), 91-109.

Messick, D. (2005). On the psychological exchange between leaders and followers. In D. M. Messick & R. M. Kramer (Eds.), The psychology of leadership: New perspectives and research (pp. 81-96). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component conceptualization of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review, 1 (1), 61 -89.

Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New York: Harper & Row.

Miller, D., & Toulouse, J. M. (1986). Strategy, structure, CEO personality and performance in small firms. American Journal of Small Business, 10(3), 47-62.

Miller, D., Kets de Vries, M. F. R., & Toulouse, J. (1982). Top executive locus of control and its relationship to strategy making, structure, and environment. Academy of Management Journal, 25(2), 237-253.

Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: The dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12(4), 177-196.

Moss, S. A., Dowling, N., & Callanan, J. (2009). Towards an integrated model of leadership and self regulation. The Leadership Quarterly, 20(2), 162-176.

Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. (1982). Employee-organization linkages: The psychology of commitment, absenteeism, and turnover. New York: Academic Press.

Mumford, M. D., Helton, W. B., Decker, B. P., Connelly, M. S., & Van Doorn, J. R. (2003). Values and beliefs related to ethical decisions. Teaching Business Ethics, 7(2), 139-170.

O’Reilly, C. A., & Chatman J. (1986). Organizational commitment and psychological attachment: The effects of compliance, identification, and internalization on prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71 (3), 492-499.

Offerman, J. (2004). When followers become toxic. Harvard Business Review, 82(1), 54-60.

Padilla, A., & Mulvey, P. (2008). Leadership Toxicity: Sources and remedies. Organisations & People, 15(3), 29-39.

Padilla, A., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2007). The toxic triangle: Destructive leaders, susceptible followers, and conducive environments. The Leadership Quarterly, 18(3), 176-194.

Palmer, T. B., & Wiseman, R. M. (1999). Decoupling risk taking from income stream uncertainty: A holistic model of risk. Strategic Management Journal, 20(11), 1037-1062.

Palmer, T., Danforth, G., & Clark, S. (1995). Strategic responses to poor performance in the health care industry: A test of competing predictions. Academy of Management Journal, 38(1), 125-129.

Paolillo, J. G. P., & Vitell, S. J. (2002). An empirical investigation of the influence of selected personal, organizational and moral intensity factors on ethical decision making. Journal of Business Ethics, 35(1), 65-74.

Paulus, D., & Christie, I. L. (1981). Spheres of control: An interactionist approach to assessment of perceived control. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research on the locus of control construct (Vol. 1, pp. 35-45). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Pearce, C. L., Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (2008). The roles of vertical and shared leadership in the enactment of executive corruption: Implications for research and practice. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(3), 353-359.

Peterson, D. K. (2003). The relationship between ethical pressure, relativistic moral beliefs and organizational commitment. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18(6), 557-572.

Pfeffer, J., & Fong, C. T. (2005). Building organization theory from first principles: The self-enhancement motive and understanding power and influence. Organization Science, 16(4), 372-388.

Pfeffer, J., & Salancik, G. R. (1978). The external control of organizations. New York: Harper & Row.

Pfeffer, J., & Sutton, R. (2006). Hard facts, half truths, and total nonsense: Profiting from evidence-based management. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Phares, E. S. (1976). Locus of control in personality. Morristown, NJ: Central Learning Press.

Pines, M. (1981). Reflections on mirroring. International Review of Psychoanalysis, 11, 27-42.

Pittenger, D. J. (2002). The two paradigms of persistence. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 128(3), 237-268.

Popper, M. (2001). Hypnotic leadership: Leaders, followers, and the loss of self. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Porr, D., & Fields, D. (2006). Implicit leadership effects on multi-source ratings for management development. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 21 (7), 651-668.

Porter, L., Steers, R., Mowday, R., & Boulian, P. (1974). Organizational commitment, job satisfaction and turnover among psychiatric technicians. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59(5), 603-609.

Quinn, R. (2003). Ethics and fundamental decisions. In N. M. Tichy & A. R. McGill (Eds.), The ethical challenge: How to lead with unyielding integrity (pp. 159-184). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Raven, B. H. (2001). Power/interaction and interpersonal influence: Experimental investigations and case studies. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & J. A. Bargh, The use and abuse of power: Multiple perspectives on the causes of corruption (pp. 217-240). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.

Rest, J. R. (1979). Development in judging moral issues. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development : Advances in research and theory. New York: Praeger.

Reicher, A. E., & Schneider, B. (1990). Climate and culture: An evolution of constructs. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 5-39). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Rhode, D. L. (2006). Introduction: Where is the leadership in moral leadership? In D. L. Rhode (Ed.), Moral leadership: The theory and practice of power, judgment, and policy (pp. 1-53). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: The Free Press.

Rosen, C. C., Levy, P. E., & Hall, R. J. (2006). Placing perceptions of politics in the context of the feedback environment, employee attitudes, and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91 (1), 211-220.

Rosenthal, S. A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2006). Narcissistic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 17(6), 617-633.

Rost, J. C. (1993). Leadership for the twenty-first century. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies of internal versus external control of reinforcements. Psychological Monographs, 80 (whole no. 609).

Rotter, J. B. (1990). Internal versus external control of reinforcement: A case history of a variable. American Psychologist, 45(4), 489-493.

Rubin, J. Z., & Brockner, J. (1975). Factors affecting entrapment in waiting situations: The Rosencrantz and Guildenstern effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 85-92.

Rusbult, C. E., Farrell, D., Rogers, G., & Mainous, A. G., III. (1988). Impact of exchange variables on exit, voice, loyalty and neglect: An integrative model of responses to declining job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 1 (3), 599-627.

Schein, E. H. (1992). Organizational culture and leadership (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Schilling, J. (2009) From ineffectiveness to destruction: A qualitative study on the meaning of negative leadership. Leadership, 5(1), 102-128.

Schminke, M., Ambrose, M. L., & Neubaum, D. O. (2005). The effect of leader moral development on ethical climate and employee attitudes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making, 9 7 (2), 135-151.

Schneider, B. (1975). Organizational climates: An essay. Personnel Psychology, 28(4), 447-479.

Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40(3), 437-454.

Schneider, B, Goldstein, H. W., & Smith, D. B. (1995). The ASA framework: An update. Personnel Psychology, 48(4), 747-773.

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theory and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1-65). New York: Academic Press.

Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the content and structure of values? Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19-45.

Schweitzer, M. E. & Gibson, D. E. (2008). Fairness, feelings, and ethical decision-making: Consequences of violating community standards of fairness. Journal of Business Ethics, 77(3), 287-301.

Schweitzer, M. E., Ordonney, L., & Douma, B. (2004). Goal-setting as a motivator of unethical behavior. Academy of Management Review, 47(3), 422-433.

Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 317-338.

Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2000). On the self-protective nature of inconsistency negativity management: Using the person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 79(6), 906-922.

Sedikides, C., & Herbst, K. (2002). How does accountability reduce self-enhancement?: The role of self-focus. Revue Internationale De Psychologie Sociale, 15(3-4), 113-128.

Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 60-79.

Sedikides, C., Herbst, K. C., Hardin, D. P., & Dardis, G. J. (2002). Accountability as a deterrent to self-enhancement: The search for mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 592-605.

Sessa, V. I., & Kaiser. (2000). Executive selection. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational effects of charismatic leadership: A self-concept based theory. Organization Science, 4, 1-17.

Singh, J. (2008). Imposters masquerading as leaders: Can the contagion be contained? Journal of Business Ethics, 82(3), 733-745.

Sitkin, S. & Pablo, A. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 9-39.

Starnes, D. M., & Zinzer, O. (1983). The effect of problem difficulty, locus of control, and sex on task persistence. The Journal of General Psychology, 108(2), 249-255.

Staw, B. M. (1976). Knee-deep in the big muddy: A study of escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 27-44.

Staw, B. M. (1981). The escalation of commitment to a course of action. Academy of Management Review, 6(4), 577-586.

Staw, B. M., & Szwajkowski, E. (1975). The scarcity munificence component of organizational environments and the commission of illegal acts. Administrative Science Quarterly, 20(3), 345-354.

Staw, B., Sandelands, L., & Dutton, J. (1981). Threat-rigidity effects in organizational behavior: A multilevel analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4), 501-524.

Steelman, L. K., Levy, P. E., & Snell, A. F. (2004). The feedback environment scale (FES): Construct definition, measurement and validation. Education and Psychological Measurement, 64, 165-184.

Street, M. (1995). Cognitive moral development and organizational commitment: Two potential predictors of whistle-blowing. Journal of Applied Business Research, 11(4), 104-110.

Street, M., & Street, V. (2006). The effects of escalating commitment on ethical decision-making. Journal of Business Ethics, 64(4), 343-356.

Street, M. D., Robertson, C., & Geiger, S. W. (1997). Ethical decision making: The effects of escalating commitment. Journal of Business Ethics, 16(11), 1153-1161.

Szwajkowski, E. (1985). Organizational illegality: Theoretical integration and illustrative application. Academy of Management Review, 10(3), 558-567.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel, & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup behavior (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

Tang, T. L., & Chiu, R. K. (2003). Income, money, ethics, pay satisfaction, commitment and unethical behavior: Is the love of money the root of evil for Hong Kong employees? Journal of Business Ethics, 46(1), 13-30.

Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Messick, D. M. (2004). Ethical fading: The role of self-deception in unethical behavior. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 223-236.

Tetlock, P. E. (1992). The impact of accountability on judgment and choice: Toward a social contingency model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 331-376). New York: Academic Press.

Treviño, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person-situation interactionist model. Academy of Management Review, 11 (3), 601-617.

Treviño, L. K., & Youngblood, S. A. (1990). Bad apples in bad barrels: A causal analysis of ethical decision-making behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(4), 378-385.

Treviño, L. K., Hartman, L. P., & Brown, M. (2000). Moral person and moral manager : How executives develop a reputation for ethical leadership. California Management Review, 42(2), 128-142.

Treviño, L., Weaver, G., Gibson, D., & Toffler, B. (1999). Managing ethics and legal compliance: What works and what hurts. California Management Review, 41(2), 131-151.

VandeWalle, D. (2003). A goal orientation model of feedback-seeking behavior. Human Resource Management Review, 13(4), 581-607.

Vardi, Y., & Weitz, E. (2004). Misbehavior in organizations: Theory, research, and management. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.

Victor, B., & Cullen, J. B. (1988). The organizational bases of ethical work climates. Administrative Science Quarterly, 33(1), 101-125.

Webley, S. (2003). Developing a code of business ethics. London, England: Institute of Business Ethics.

Webley, S., & Werner, A. (2008). Corporate codes of ethics: Necessary but not sufficient. Business Ethics, 17(4), 405-415.

Whitaker, B., Dahling, J., & Levy, P. (2007). The development of a feedback environment and role clarity model of job performance. Journal of Management, 33(4), 570-591.

Whicker, M. L. (1996). Toxic leaders: When organizations go bad. Westport, CT: Quorum Books.

Whyte, G. (1986). Escalating commitment to a course of action: A reinterpretation. Academy of Management Review, 11 (2), 311-321.

Whyte, G. (1991). Diffusion of responsibility: Effects on the escalation tendency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(3), 408-415.

Whyte, G., Saks, A. M., & Hook, S. (1997). When success breeds failure: The role of self efficacy in escalating commitment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18(5), 415-432.

Yun, S., Takeuchi, R., & Liu, W. (2007). Employee self-enhancement motives and job performance behaviors: investigating the moderating effects of employee role ambiguity and managerial perceptions of employee commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(3), 745-756.

Ziglar-Hill, V. (2006). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit self-esteem: Implications for narcissism and self-esteem instability. Journal of Personality, 74(1), 119-143.

Zimbardo, P. (1969). The psychology of evil: A situationist perspective on recruiting good people to engage in anti-social acts. Research in Social Psychology, 11, 125-133.

Zimbardo, P. (2006). On the psychology of power: To the person? To the situation, To the system? In D. Rhode (Ed.), Moral leadership: The theory and practice of power, judgment, and policy (pp. 129-157). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Zimbardo, P. (2007). The Lucifer Effect: Understanding how good people turn evil. New York: Random House.

Zimbardo, P. G., Maslach, C., & Haney, C. (1999). Reflections on the Stanford Prison Experiment: Genesis, transformations, consequences. In T. Blass (Ed.), Obedience to authority: Current perspectives on the Milgram Paradigm (pp. 193-237). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.