CHAPTER 12


ANIMAL MODELS OF PRENATAL PROTEIN MALNUTRITION RELEVANT FOR SCHIZOPHRENIA

LISA M. TARANTINO, TERESA M. REYES, AND ABRAHAM A. PALMER

KEY CONCEPTS

Exposure to a variety of environmental challenges in utero has been shown to elevate risk for schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders; for reviews, see Weiss & Feldon (2001) and Meyer & Feldon (2009). In particular, retrospective studies in human populations support a link between prenatal exposure to nutritional deficiency and an increased risk for development of schizophrenia (Susser & Lin, 1992; Susser et al., 1996; St Clair et al., 2005; Brown & Susser, 2008; Xu et al., 2009; see chapter 2, this book). A large natural experiment on prenatal nutrition in humans occurred at the end of World War II. A Nazi-imposed blockade that affected the six largest cities in the Netherlands resulted in a famine called the Dutch Hunger Winter. During the height of the famine, daily food rations for all residents were limited to 1,000 calories and eventually to 500 calories. Retrospective studies identified neuropsychiatric and other disorders in the offspring of women exposed to the Dutch famine during gestation. Adults conceived during the famine had an increased risk for schizophrenia (Hoek, Brown, & Susser, 1998; Brown & Susser, 2008) and major affective disorder (Brown et al., 1995, 2000). Similar studies of offspring that were in utero during the Chinese famine of 1959–1961 confirmed an increased prevalence of schizophrenia (St Clair et al., 2005; Xu et al., 2009).

It is impossible to use human subjects to identify the causative nutrient or nutrients or to establish a mechanism for the role of prenatal nutrition in schizophrenia or other psychiatric disorders because of the obvious practical and ethical challenges. Animal models provide an alternative means to study the role of gestational environment, and particularly malnutrition, on brain development and adult behavior.

Neurodevelopmental Hypothesis of Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a complex etiology resulting from both genetic and environmental factors. Evidence suggests that environmental insults acting on individuals with an existing genetic predisposition can cause developmental alterations that are present long before the onset of psychotic symptoms. Research has identified at least two windows of developmental vulnerability: the prenatal or perinatal period and early adolescence. Animal models offer the advantage of manipulating the environment at multiple points during development and measuring outcomes across the life span. Because schizophrenia symptoms normally appear in late adolescence or early adulthood, brain and behavioral abnormalities that are observed only in adult and not young animals may provide particularly attractive models for further study.

A number of early environment insults that are linked to increased risk for schizophrenia in humans have also been modeled in animals. Among the most replicated include maternal infection, maternal stress, obstetric complications, and maternal dietary malnutrition (both vitamin D and protein deprivation). These are reviewed in other chapters in this book. The present chapter provides a detailed review of early life protein malnutrition in animal models and discusses the behavioral, neurochemical, and neurophysiologic effects in relation to schizophrenia.

Animal models of nutritional deficiency have produced a number of phenotypic outcomes, including metabolic, cardiovascular, and mental disorders, among others (for a review see Armitage et al., 2004). Dietary manipulations in animals can be limited to a particular time during development, in adulthood, or across the life span. A review of all of these models would be beyond the scope of a single chapter. Instead, we limit our discussion to protein malnutrition that occurs prenatally and perinatally or postnatally. Maternal protein deprivation is defined as a dietary manipulation in females that begins before breeding and that can end at any time during pregnancy or extend through gestation and weaning into adulthood. Perinatal or postnatal protein deprivation is defined as dietary manipulations that begin during mid-gestation or immediately at birth and can last throughout weaning, into adulthood, or both. Development in rodents lags behind human development, and many relevant in utero developmental processes in humans occur in the postnatal period in rodents (Morgane et al., 1993; Clancy, Darlington, & Finlay, 2001). Therefore, studies intended to examine the entire early brain development period in humans should include protein deprivation in the early postnatal period as well as during embryogenesis in the rodent.

In animal models, protein deprivation normally is achieved by modifying the amount of casein in the diet while maintaining the caloric value by the addition of extra carbohydrates. The fraction of protein in a deficient diet ranges from 5–9%, whereas protein in normal diets ranges from 16–25%.

Protein Malnutrition and Effects on the Developing Nervous System

Most neurotransmitter systems have been implicated in schizophrenia on the basis of functional characteristics; the efficacy of receptor modulators (i.e., pharmacological manipulation with agonists and antagonists) to ameliorate, exacerbate, or mimic symptoms of the disease; or both. Evidence for a functional role of the dopaminergic, glutamatergic, serotonergic, and GABAergic systems (those that transmit or secrete gamma-aminobutyric acid) has been reviewed in detail elsewhere (Stone, Morrison, & Pilowsky, 2007; Toda & Abi-Dargham, 2007; Geyer & Vollenweider, 2008; Jones et al., 2008; Paz et al., 2008), and efforts toward understanding the effects of protein deprivation during development on each system have been assessed in animal models.

Structural Changes

Structural changes in the brain have been observed in schizophrenia, including increased ventricular volume and decreased whole brain volume (Hulshoff Pol & Kahn, 2008; Fatemi & Folsom, 2009). Although reduced brain and body weight have been reported in animals that were exposed to low protein in utero (Smart et al., 1973; Marichich, Molina, & Orsingher, 1979), gross structural abnormalities have not been observed. Rather, the deficits resulting from prenatal protein malnutrition tend to be subtle and relate to deficits within specific neurotransmitter systems, although some generalized abnormalities have been reported. For example, exposure to prenatal protein deficiency for the first two weeks of gestation in rats results in significant abnormalities in brain development in newborn pups, including delayed astrocytogenesis, abnormal neuronal differentiation (as shown by reduced microtubule-associated protein-5 [MAP-5] and increased MAP-1 expression), abnormal synaptogenesis, and decreased apoptosis (Gressens et al., 1997). It is important to note, however, that all of these deficits are normalized by adulthood (postnatal day 63). A subtle, region-specific change that has been consistently noted in response to prenatal protein malnutrition is a reduction in cell number in the hippocampus. Because the anatomy and circuitry of the hippocampus is well defined, the effect of prenatal protein malnutrition on this structure has been the focus of numerous groups. A reduced number of cells in CA1 (but no other hippocampal regions) and a reduced subiculum volume have been reported (Lister et al., 2005), whereas other groups have found changes in cell number more broadly across the dentate gyrus, CA1 and CA3, extensively detailed previously (Díaz-Cintra et al., 1991; Díaz-Cintra et al., 1994; Cintra et al., 1997a; Cintra et al., 1997b). Beyond these few notable deficits, numerous subtle changes in neurotransmitter expression and function have been identified. Yet these findings are complex and can vary in direction across different brain regions or assessment time points.

Dopamine

All currently prescribed antipsychotic medications have an affinity for dopamine (DA) D2 receptors. The long-standing DA dysfunction hypothesis of schizophrenia has been supported by the observation of increases in striatal DA D2 receptor binding and differences in DA dynamics in the brains of schizophrenic patients (Seeman et al., 1987; Joyce et al., 1988; Howes et al., 2009). An increase in D2 receptor binding in the striatum and a decrease in DA transporter binding have also been reported in rats exposed to prenatal protein deprivation (Palmer et al., 2008). Studies examining DA release and tissue concentration in the hippocampus of protein-deprived animals have been less consistent. Chen et al. (1995) reported an increased release of DA and its metabolites in hippocampal slices from prenatally protein malnourished animals, and a decrease in DA was detected in the hippocampus in prenatally malnourished animals (Kehoe et al., 2001). A decrease in basal DA was also detected (by microdialysis) in the prefrontal cortex of prenatally malnourished animals, tested as adults, as well as an absence of restraint stress-induced release of DA (Mokler et al., 2007). However, in a separate study, the expected increase in hypothalamic DA in response to isolation stress was intact in the prenatally malnourished animals tested as adults (Kehoe et al., 2001).

Serotonin (5-HT)

Serotonin dysfunction, in particular of the 5-HT2A receptor, is implicated in some symptoms of schizophrenia. This implication is based on the ability of 5-HT2A agonists, such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) or psilocybin, to induce psychotic symptoms, working memory deficits, and sensorimotor gating abnormalities (Vollenweider et al., 1998; Umbricht et al., 2003). Atypical antipsychotics also have a higher affinity for 5-HT2A receptors than D2 receptors (Garzya et al., 2007).

Serotonergic changes in protein-deprived animals generally are consistent across studies. Increased 5-HT or its metabolite, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), or both are detected in the hippocampus (via microdialysis or in tissue) (Kehoe et al., 2001; Mokler, Galle, & Morgane, 2003; Mokler et al., 2007); throughout the brain (Resnick & Morgane, 1984); in hippocampal slices (Chen et al., 1995); and in the hypothalamus (Kehoe et al., 2001). Further, in response to stress, an increased release is detected in the hippocampus, as opposed to a decrease in control animals (Mokler et al., 2007). Animals from prenatally malnourished dams also show an altered response to dl-fenfluramine, which blocks the 5-HT transporter and stimulates 5-HT release. These offspring show a decreased behavioral response (inhibition of food intake) and reduced Fos immunoreactivity in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus in response to dl-fenfluramine (Lopes de Souza et al., 2008). Another study also detected 5-HT abnormalities within the hippocampus, including a decrease in 5-HT fiber density in the dentate gyrus and CA3; decreased 5-HT uptake in CA3 and CA1; and decreased expression of the 5-HT1A receptor in CA3 (Blatt et al., 1994). Contrary to the other reports, however, these authors did not find an increase in 5-HT within the hippocampus.

Glutamate

N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor blockers like phencyclidine (PCP), ketamine, and dizocipline (MK-801) produce psychotomimetic effects in healthy individuals and precipitate psychotic episodes in schizophrenia patients (Large, 2007). In addition, NMDA receptor and mGluR2/3 agonists are effective in treating schizophrenia (Javitt, 2006; Patil et al., 2007). Therefore, glutamatergic dysfunction, and particularly NMDA receptor hypofunction, is hypothesized to play a role in schizophrenia.

Abnormalities in the glutamate system have been detected in protein-deprived animals. For example, increased MK-801 binding is observed in the adult cortex, striatum, and hippocampus of rats deprived of protein prenatally, indicating decreased glutamatergic activity (Palmer et al., 2004; Palmer et al., 2008). Increased kainate receptor binding is also detected in the CA3 of animals deprived of protein prenatally. The authors speculate that this may be a compensation for reduced glutamatergic input from mossy fibers (Fiacco et al., 2003). Further, decreased sensitivity to quinolinic acid (an NMDA agonist) was detected in prenatally malnourished adult animals (Schweigert et al., 2005).

Gamma-aminobutyric Acid

GABAergic interneurons are a core component of the circuitry that controls network oscillations, information processing, and sensorimotor gating—all of which are disturbed in schizophrenia (Benes & Berretta, 2001). Therefore, alterations of the GABAergic system have been examined in protein-deprived animals as a functional model for symptoms of schizophrenia.

Defects in the GABA system have been detected in adult rodents that were protein-deprived in utero. An increase in the expression of GAD-67, a rate-limiting enzyme in GABA synthesis, is detected in the dentate gyrus of prenatally malnourished animals (Díaz-Cintra et al., 2007). Alterations in the expression of GABA-A receptors are also detected in rats that were protein-restricted prenatally, including decreased GABA-A γ2L mRNA in the septum (Steiger et al., 2002), decreased GABA-A (alpha 1, beta 2) in the hippocampus, and an increase in alpha 3 expression in the hippocampus (Steiger et al., 2003). Higher GABA uptake by cortical and hippocampal slices is also observed in malnourished rats (Schweigert et al., 2005).

Effects of Prenatal Protein Malnutrition on Rodent Behavior

Assessing the effects of prenatal protein deprivation in animals requires the use of appropriate behavioral, neurochemical, and physiological assays that can accurately reflect human schizophrenia symptomatology. An animal model encompassing the entire spectrum of schizophrenia symptoms is impossible. Instead, the focus of animal research has been to model specific symptoms of the disease by examining endophenotypes. Human behavioral symptoms of schizophrenia can be grouped into two general categories: positive (e.g., hallucinations, delusions) and negative (e.g., social withdrawal, working memory deficits). Currently available medications for schizophrenia are more effective at treating positive rather than negative symptoms (Javitt et al., 2008). Animal models for both positive and negative symptoms exist and have been used widely in rodents (table 12.1). In addition, endophenotypes, which are traits that are enriched in affected individuals but are not symptoms of the disease, are also useful for developing animal models (Gottesman & Gould, 2003). Specific endophenotypes have been developed for schizophrenia. Models of disease symptoms and endophenotypes should provide face, predictive, and construct validity (Geyer & Markou, 1995). Many behavioral models of schizophrenia respond to human pharmacotherapies and are modulated by the same brain regions and neurotransmitter pathways (Li et al., 2009).

Table 12.1 Commonly Used Animal Models of Schizophrenia Endophenotypes and Human Correlates

image

EEG = electroencephalogram; ICSS = intracranial self-stimulation; PPI = prepulse inhibition.

SOURCE: Authors.

Numerous studies have reported on the behavioral effects of prenatal, perinatal, or postnatal protein deprivation in adult offspring. Table 12.2 lists most of the studies that have examined the behavioral phenotype resulting from protein deprivation in utero, with a focus on animal models of behaviors that model symptoms or endophenotypes (or both) of schizophrenia.

Prepulse Inhibition

Behavioral and physiologic models of schizophrenia have focused mainly on negative symptoms and endophenotypes. Sensorimotor gating is one of the most widely studied endophenotypes. Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is a well-validated neurophysiologic test of sensorimotor gating that has almost identical human behavioral correlates and responds to atypical antipsychotics in both human (reviewed in Braff, Geyer, & Swerdlow, 2001; Swerdlow et al., 2008) and animal studies (Geyer et al., 2001; Swerdlow et al., 2008). Schizophrenia patients and asymptomatic first-degree relatives show deficits in PPI. In animal models, these deficits can be replicated by both developmental and pharmacological manipulations that affect neurotransmitter pathways and brain regions involved in the disease—most notably by DA agonists (Swerdlow et al., 1994; Geyer et al., 2001), 5-HT agonists (Dulawa et al., 2000), NMDA receptor antagonists (Geyer et al., 2001), GABA antagonists (Swerdlow et al., 1990), maternal infection and immune activation (Patterson, 2009; chapter 10, this book), maternal stress (chapter 13, this book) and neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions (Lipska et al., 1995). PPI is also disrupted in other disorders, including Alzheimer’s disease (Ueki et al., 2006), bipolar disorder (Perry et al., 2001) and obsessive compulsive disorder (Shanahan et al., 2009), indicating that this behavioral deficit is not specific to schizophrenia.

Despite the widespread use of PPI as an endophenotype for schizophrenia (Swerdlow et al., 2008), only one study has been published that examined PPI following prenatal protein malnutrition. Palmer et al. (2004) reported that protein deprivation in utero is associated with lower PPI in young adult female rat offspring at postnatal day (PND) 56. However, no such deficit is observed in young adult males in this experiment. Moreover, PPI deficits are not observed in adolescent (PND 35) male or female rats exposed to maternal protein deprivation. The significant decrease in PPI in young adult female rats exposed to protein deprivation in utero is consistent with the adult onset of schizophrenia and supports the role of prenatal protein malnutrition in the development of this particular endophenotype.

Alterations in startle reactivity have been reported in patients with schizophrenia (Geyer & Braff, 1982; Braff, Grillon, & Geyer, 1992) and related disorders (Cadenhead, Geyer, & Braff, 1993), and are hypothesized to reflect a defect in central inhibitory mechanisms. Palmer et al. (2004) also observed a decrease in initial startle response in protein-deprived rats. However, a more recent study reported no 1973 difference in startle response among protein-deprived versus control rats (Francolin-Silva, Brandao, & Almeida, 2007). There are several technical differences between this and the Palmer et al. study. Whereas Francolin-Silva limited protein malnutrition to the perinatal period, from birth up to PND 28, Palmer used a prenatal protein deprivation model that started 5 weeks before mating, lasting until PND 0. Also, the startle response amplitude measure used by Francolin-Silva, Brandao, & Almeida (2007) was averaged over 30 trials, whereas Palmer et al. (2004) observed a difference in startle response only in the initial presentations of the startle stimulus.

Table 12.2 Behavioral Changes in Adult Rodents After Prenatal, Perinatal, or Postnatal Exposure to Low Protein Levels

image

image

image

image

image

image

image

image

AMPH = amphetamine; BLA = basolateral amygdala; DOPAC = 3,4-Dihydroxyphenylacetic acid; Emb-LPD = Embryonic Low Protein Diet; EPM = Elevated Plus Maze; ETM = Elevated T-Maze; EtOH = Ethanol; FosB = FBJ murine osteosarcoma viral oncogene homolog B; LP = Low Protein; LPD = Low Protein Diet; NA = Nucleus Accumbens; REM = rapid eye movement; PND = postnatal day.

SOURCE: Authors.

Cognitive Function

Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia largely drive functional outcomes for the disease and are not well managed by current antipsychotics (Fenton, Stover, & Insel, 2003). Appreciation of cognitive deficits as a core feature of the disease has generated an increased effort toward identification of appropriate animal models for screening novel pharmaceuticals for this disease domain. A National Institutes of Mental Health initiative, Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS), has identified seven primary cognitive domains that are affected in human schizophrenia (Green et al., 2004). A number of these cognitive features are readily measured in rodents by using basic learning and memory tasks that assess working memory, attention, visual learning, and social cognition (table 12.1). Deficits in many of these cognitive assays are observed in lesion studies of the prefrontal cortex that replicate brain structure abnormalities found in human patients and in animals genetically modified to express disease susceptibility genes, thus providing encouraging evidence for their usefulness in modeling the human disease (reviewed in Arguello & Gogos, 2009, and Kellendonk, Simpson, & Kandel, 2009). A desire to understand potential developmental causes for the cognitive deficits observed in schizophrenia patients drives research on learning and memory deficits in protein-deprived animals.

Protein deprivation during development causes lasting changes in brain regions implicated in learning and memory (Morgane et al., 1993). Perhaps one of the simplest assays of sensory and cognitive development is the homing test in which suckling animals are tested for their ability to locate the nest after displacement. Data have consistently shown that protein-malnourished animals are deficient in the ability to return to the nest (Gallo, Werboff, & Knox, 1984; Galler, Tonkiss, & Maldonado-Irizarry, 1994; Tonkiss, Harrison, & Galler, 1996).

Protein deprivation in utero or during the suckling period causes significant hippocampal alterations, including long-term potentiation deficits (Austin, Bronzino, & Morgane, 1986). The hippocampus plays a critical role in the ability to learn and remember spatial information. Tests of learning and memory indicate that prenatally malnourished rats are behaviorally inflexible (Tonkiss et al., 1993) and often show no deficits in acquisition, while being unable or unwilling to give up learned responses once acquired. For instance, prenatal protein deprivation does not affect acquisition of the alternation response in a spatial T-maze task (Goodlett et al., 1986; Tonkiss & Galler, 1990) or an operant model of the T-maze task (Tonkiss & Galler, 1990), spatial navigation in the Morris water maze (Goodlett et al., 1986; Tonkiss, Shultz, & Galler, 1994), or performance in the radial arm maze (Hall, 1983). However, rats deprived of protein in utero require more sessions to abolish learned alternation responses (Tonkiss & Galler, 1990). In conditioned taste aversion studies, prenatally malnourished rats show impaired extinction of the conditioned taste aversion response with repeated exposure (Tonkiss et al., 1993). Following continuous reinforcement in an operant task, prenatally malnourished rats exhibit impairments in acquiring a differential reinforcement of low rates of responding (DRL) task (Tonkiss et al., 1990a). These data imply that prenatal protein deprivation does not alter the ability to learn but rather makes it harder to “unlearn” once an association has been made.

There has been one report of radial arm maze performance in mice that were protein-deprived prenatally. Ranade et al. (2008) found that outbred Swiss mice make more reference memory and working memory errors—an interesting result that encourages further study of mice as animal models for the cognitive effects of prenatal protein deprivation.

Social Interaction

Impaired social interactions are a hallmark of schizophrenia, and improvement of social skills is an important element in treatment and outcomes for individuals with the disease. Two studies of social interaction behaviors in protein-malnourished rats reported a deficit in several social behaviors (Frankova, 1973; Almeida et al., 1996c). Both of these studies were performed in juvenile rats but differed in the protein deprivation protocol. Almeida and colleagues deprived prenatally until birth, whereas Frankova deprived from birth until PND 49. Contradictory results have emerged when adult offspring from dams that experienced total caloric undernutrition, as opposed to only protein malnutrition, were tested. In these studies, adult rats display increased social interaction and more aggression rather than a decrease in social interaction (Whatson, Smart, & Dobbing, 1975, 1976; Tonkiss & Smart, 1983). The extent to which age of testing, developmental period of malnutrition, or type of nutritional deficiency (overall caloric malnutrition or protein deprivation) explains these contradictory results has yet to be resolved. However, the limited results from protein-deprived juvenile rats suggest that such an experimental paradigm produces social behaviors that mimic those seen in human schizophrenia.

Sleep Abnormalities

Difficulties initiating or maintaining sleep occur in 30–80% of individuals with schizophrenia (Cohrs, 2008). Several studies have examined the result of prenatal protein deprivation on circadian and sleep behaviors. Measurement of 24-hour locomotor activity indicates that, although rats deprived of protein in utero are not impaired in spontaneous locomotor activity and show circadian rhythmicity, they do display an advanced phase shift in locomotor activity (Castanon-Cervantes & Cintra, 2002; Duran et al., 2005). Protein-deprived rats show peak activity levels before the onset of the light phase, whereas control animals show peak activity after the onset of the light phase. Duran et al. (2005) observed the phase shift in locomotor activity using standard light-dark conditions in 88-day-old rats, whereas Castanon-Cervantes and Cintra (2002) observed the phase shift in dark-dark conditions but not under a light-dark light cycle and in younger (55 days of age) but not older (550 days of age) rats. In addition, Duran and colleagues used only females, and the sex of the rats used in the Castanon-Cervantes study is not apparent from the publication.

The suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the hypothalamus is the master pacemaker for the generation and maintenance of circadian rhythms, and abnormalities in SCN development have been noted in rat pups from protein-deprived dams. Immunohistochemical examination of vasoactive intestinal peptide and arginine vasopressin neurons within the SCN revealed altered density and morphology of these two systems within this nucleus (Rojas et al., 2009).

Undernutrition-driven changes in sleep behavior are a bit more complex, but several studies indicate that the amplitude of the rhythm for both wakefulness and rapid eye movement sleep is advanced in the dark phase of the light cycle in rats that were deprived of protein in utero (Cintra et al., 2002; Duran et al., 2006) and that these changes are exacerbated by acute stress (Duran et al., 2006). A thorough review of stress and stress-related disorders and the relationship to psychiatric disorders was recently presented (Chrousos, 2009). In summary, prenatal protein malnutrition in animal models appears to alter both circadian rhythms and the sleep-wake cycle. This alteration occurs primarily at the time of light change and may provide insight into the mechanisms of sleep disturbances in schizophrenia.

Behavioral Responses to Pharmacological Challenges

Dopamine

Examination of DA-mediated behaviors in animals that were protein-deprived during development reveals lasting changes in the DA system. Most studies examining DA agonists have reported increased locomotor activity in response to amphetamine (Brioni et al., 1986; Palmer et al., 2008) and cocaine (Shultz, Galler, & Tonkiss, 1999; Valdomero et al., 2005), as well as increased stereotypy in response to apomorphine (Leahy et al., 1978; Palmer et al., 2008). Some of these differences are found in young adult female but not young adult male or adolescent rats of either sex (Palmer et al., 2008). An increase in DA release in the nucleus accumbens core following cocaine administration in cocaine-sensitized, protein-deprived rats has also been observed (Valdomero et al., 2005). These observations are consistent with the increased sensitivity to psychostimulants observed in schizophrenia (Geyer & Markou, 1995; van den Buuse et al., 2005) and indicate that prenatal protein deprivation might be a good model for DA dysfunction in human schizophrenia.

Serotonin

In behavioral studies, the 5-HT2 receptor antagonist ritanserin decreases anxiety in the open field in postnatally protein-deprived rats but had no effect on control animals (Almeida, de Oliveira, & Graeff, 1990). Postnatally deprived rats also show decreased sensitivity to the 5-HT2A receptor antagonists, 5-methoxy-dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT) and dimethyltryptamine (DMT), in rotarod and treadmill assays, and for DMT-induced behaviors (Hall, Leahy, & Robertson, 1983; Keller et al., 1994).

Gamma-aminobutyric Acid

Benzodiazepines are positive allosteric modulators of the GABA-A receptor that are commonly used as anxiolytics but also have amnesic and sedative properties. Animals that have been exposed to low protein at any time during development show a reduced sensitivity to the effects of benzodiazepines. This result has been observed repeatedly in numerous animal models of anxiety, including the elevated plus maze (Almeida, de Oliveira, & Graeff, 1991; Cordoba et al., 1992; Laino, Cordoba, & Orsingher, 1993; Santucci et al., 1994; Borghese et al., 1998; Francolin-Silva et al., 2007), light-dark (Brioni & Orsingher, 1988; Santucci et al., 1994), and fear-potentiated startle tests (Francolin-Silva & Almeida, 2004).

Rats deprived of protein prenatally also show a decreased sensitivity to the amnesic effects of chlordiazepoxide in the Morris water maze (Tonkiss et al., 2000) but show an increased behavioral sensitivity to the stimulus properties of chlordiazepoxide (Shultz, Galler, & Tonkiss, 2002), indicating that protein deprivation differentially affects other aspects of benzodiazepine action.

Drug Abuse

Drug abuse has been linked to the development of schizophrenia and, as described above, protein-deprived animals show an elevated locomotor response to psychostimulants such as cocaine, amphetamine, and apomorphine. However, there are also reports that these animals are more sensitive to the rewarding effects of cocaine (Valdomero et al., 2006) and show an increase in response for other rewarding substances, such as morphine (Valdomero et al., 2007), food (Tonkiss et al., 1990b), sweetened milk (Brioni & Orsingher, 1988), and saccharin (Tonkiss et al., 1990b), indicating that protein deprivation during development alters drug reward circuitry and may be a useful environmental manipulation for studying the role of drug reward and abuse in the development of schizophrenia.

Anxiety-Related Behaviors

Many studies have examined the role of prenatal protein malnutrition on anxiety-related behaviors. Although these behaviors are not considered to be models of schizophrenia, there is significant comorbidity between anxiety and schizophrenia that may indicate a shared liability (Buckley et al., 2009). In any case, the data on anxiety phenotypes in protein-malnourished animals provide further insight into the behavioral effects of this environmental manipulation. In general, animals exposed to protein malnutrition during early development exhibit reduced anxiety in the elevated plus maze (Almeida, de Oliveira, & Graeff, 1991; Almeida, Tonkiss, & Galler, 1996b; Francolin-Silva & Almeida, 2004), the elevated T-maze (Almeida, Tonkiss, & Galler, 1996a; Hernandes & Almeida, 2003), and the fear-potentiated startle test (Francolin-Silva, Brandao, & Almeida, 2007). An exception is the report of Borghese et al. (1998), which found a decrease in the percentage of time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze. However, in this experiment the animals received a daily injection of vehicle for 15 days before testing. It is possible that either the vehicle or the chronic stress of receiving daily injections had an effect on behavior in the elevated plus maze. Stress is known to interact with prenatal malnutrition to modify behavior. For instance, acute immobilization stress reverses the anxiolytic behavior observed in the elevated plus maze (Francolin-Silva & Almeida, 2004), and exposure to chronic stress reverses some of the anxiolytic and antidepressant-like effects of prenatal protein deprivation (Trzctnska, Tonkiss, & Galler, 1999).

In contrast to the assays already described here, assessment of protein-deprived animals in a different test of anxiety, the open field, has shown increased (Trzctnska, Tonkiss, & Galler, 1999; Watkins et al., 2008b), decreased (Watkins et al., 2008a), or no change in anxiety behaviors (Brioni et al., 1986). Differences among the methods used in these studies do exist and could explain the contradictory results. It is also important to note that the studies by Watkins used mice, whereas all other studies of the effects of protein malnutrition on anxiety-related behaviors have used rats. Obviously, differences in the effects of protein deprivation between mice and rats are possible, and additional studies are needed to understand the behavioral effects of low protein during development in mice.

Overall, most studies of anxiety-related behavior indicate decreased anxiety in animals that have been subjected to protein deprivation in utero, but further research is required to gain a better understanding of the mechanisms that control these behavioral responses.

The Role of Epigenetics

Epigenetic mechanisms have also been proposed as possible mediators of the effects of prenatal protein deprivation on downstream phenotypes (see chapter 9, this book). Epigenetics is defined as inherited phenotypic variations that are not the result of variations in the DNA sequence. This includes DNA methylation, histone modifications, and genomic imprinting, which can occur both prenatally and postnatally and can establish either transient or long-lasting changes in gene expression.

The role of diet in DNA methylation is significant. DNA methylation is catalyzed by DNA methyltransferases that transfer methyl groups from S-adenosylmethionine (SAM) to cytosine in CpG islands. The SAM required for methylation comes, in part, from dietary methyl group intake, and the major source of methyl groups in food is methionine. If methionine is limited in the diet, the amount of SAM available is also limited and hypomethylation is found (Wainfan et al., 1989; Vachtenheim, Horakova, & Novotna, 1994). However, there also are de novo pathways (one-carbon metabolism) and other dietary sources of methyl groups, including choline, folic acid, and vitamin B12 (Niculescu & Zeisel, 2002). Limiting methionine in the diet during development is likely to result in some compensatory changes in these other pathways so that some level of methylation can occur.

Perhaps the most dramatic example of the effects of dietary methyl groups is represented by the observation that dietary supplementation of methyl groups in pregnant black pseudoagouti (Avy/a) dams alters epigenetic regulation of agouti expression by increasing methylation at the long terminal repeat, resulting in increased agouti-black mottling in offspring (Wolff et al., 1998; Cooney, Dave, & Wolff, 2002). Gene-specific hypomethylation of peroxisomal proliferator-activated receptor alpha (Lillycrop et al., 2008) and the glucocorticoid receptor (Lillycrop et al., 2005) has also been observed in the offspring of rodents fed a low-protein diet, and this can be prevented with folic acid supplementation. It is interesting that methyl deficiency causing global DNA hypomethylation can occur simultaneously with gene-specific hypomethylation or hypermethylation (Singh, Murphy, & O’Reilly, 2003). A methyl-deficient diet also modifies histone methylation and increases expression of both immunoglobulin factor 2 (Igf2) and H19 genes, indicating that at some loci, methyl deficiency may result in chromatin modification. It is important that hypomethylation of Igf2 is also found in DNA samples from humans who were exposed to the Dutch famine during development (Heijmans et al., 2008).

Recent technical advances are opening up the possibility of measuring changes in methylation status on a genomewide scale (Schumacher et al., 2006). Allele-specific methylation using array-based technology allows tracking of both gene and allele-specific changes in methylation. Paired with an experimental genetic population of animals, this technique presents a powerful tool for examining the role of prenatal nutritional environment on allele-specific methylation status and its effects on behavior.

Conclusions and Future Directions

The spectrum of data presented here reflects years of research on a variety of animal models, including work from many diverse disciplines that suggests that protein deprivation during gestation, immediately after birth, or both produces behavioral, neurochemical, and developmental changes that accurately mirror many of the symptoms of human schizophrenia.

The challenge moving forward is to integrate these environmentally induced behavioral, neurochemical, and developmental changes with the relevant genetic factors that increase susceptibility to developing schizophrenia. Numerous genes and gene expression changes have been studied in schizophrenia; for reviews, see Jones et al. (2008) and Desbonnet, Waddington, & O’Tuathaigh, (2009a, 2009b). Animal models of these can be subjected to protein deprivation to look for gene-environment interaction.

The identification of specific genetic and environmental causes of schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders remains a challenge for the research community. Findings reviewed in this chapter highlight the importance of maternal nutrition, specifically adequate protein nutrition, within the prenatal, perinatal, and early postnatal developmental critical periods. The availability of tools for directly assessing the impact of environmental manipulations on gene structure and expression, along with a greater appreciation for the role of gene-environment interactions in complex disease, has the potential to move the field forward in the next decade.

KEY AREAS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH

Acknowledgments

The authors’ work is supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA022392 and DA023690 to L. M. Tarantino) and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH087978 and MH091372 to T. M. Reyes and MH079103 to A. A. Palmer).

Selected Readings

Meyer, U. & Feldon, J. (2009). Epidemiology-driven neurodevelopmental animal models of schizophrenia. Progress in Neurobiology 90(3): 285–326.

Morgane, P. J., Austin-LaFrance, R., Bronzino, J., Tonkiss, J., Diaz-Cintra, S., Cintra, L., Kemper, T., & Galler, J. R. (1993). Prenatal malnutrition and development of the brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 17(1): 91–128.

Rutten, B. P. & Mill, J. (2009). Epigenetic mediation of environmental influences in major psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35(6): 1045–1056.

Tonkiss, J., Galler, J., Morgane, P. J., Bronzino, J. D., & Austin-LaFrance, R. J. (1993). Prenatal protein malnutrition and postnatal brain function. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 678: 215–227.

van Os, J. & Rutten, B. P. (2009). Gene-environment-wide interaction studies in psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry 166(9): 964–966.

References

Almeida, S. S., de Oliveira, L. M., & Graeff, F. G. (1990). Decreased reactivity to anxiolytics caused by early protein malnutrition in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 36(4): 997–1000.

Almeida, S. S., de Oliveira, L. M., & Graeff, F. G. (1991). Early life protein malnutrition changes exploration of the elevated plus-maze and reactivity to anxiolytics. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 103(4): 513–518.

Almeida, S. S., Garcia, R. A., & de Oliveira, L. M. (1993). Effects of early protein malnutrition and repeated testing upon locomotor and exploratory behaviors in the elevated plus-maze. Physiology & Behavior 54(2): 749–752.

Almeida, S. S., Soares, E. G., Bichuette, M. Z., Graeff, F. G., & de Oliveira, L. M. (1992). Effects of early postnatal malnutrition and chlordiazepoxide on experimental aversive situations. Physiology & Behavior 51(6): 1195–1199.

Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J., & Galler, J. R. (1996a). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects avoidance but not escape behavior in the elevated T-maze test. Physiology and Behavior 60(1): 191–195.

Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J., & Galler, J. R. (1996b). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects exploratory behavior of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiology and Behavior 60(2): 675–680.

Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J., & Galler, J. R. (1996c). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects the social interactions of juvenile rats. Physiology and Behavior 60(1): 197–201.

Amitai, N., Semenova, S., & Markou, A. (2009). Clozapine attenuates disruptions in response inhibition and task efficiency induced by repeated phencyclidine administration in the intracranial self-stimulation procedure. European Journal of Pharmacology 602(1): 78–84.

Arguello, P. A. & Gogos, J. A. (2009). Cognition in mouse models of schizophrenia susceptibility genes. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36(2): 289–300.

Armitage, J., Khan, I., Taylor, P., Nathanielsz, P., & Poston, L. (2004). Developmental programming of the metabolic syndrome by maternal nutritional imbalance: How strong is the evidence from experimental models in mammals? Journal of Physiology 561(2): 355–377.

Audet, M. C., Goulet, S., & Dore, F. Y. (2006). Repeated subchronic exposure to phencyclidine elicits excessive atypical grooming in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 167(1): 103–110.

Austin, K. B., Bronzino, J., & Morgane, P. J. (1986). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects synaptic potentiation in the dentate gyrus of rats in adulthood. Brain Research 394(2): 267–273.

Belforte, J. E., Zsiros, V., Sklar, E. R., Jiang, Z., Yu, G., Li, Y., Quinlan, E. M., & Nakazawa, K. (2010). Postnatal NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Nature Neuroscience 13(1): 76–83.

Bellinger, L., Sculley, D. V., Langley-Evans, S. C. (2006). Exposure to undernutrition in fetal life determines fat distribution, locomotor activity and food intake in ageing rats. International Journal of Obesity (Lond) 30(5): 729–738.

Benes, F. M. & Berretta, S. (2001). GABAergic interneurons: Implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 25(1): 1–27.

Berlin, I., Givry-Steiner, L., Lecrubier, Y., & Puech, A. J. (1998). Measures of anhedonia and hedonic responses to sucrose in depressive and schizophrenic patients in comparison with healthy subjects. European Psychiatry 13(6): 303–309.

Blatt, G. J., Chen, J. C., Rosene, D. L., Volicer, L., & Galler, J. R. (1994). Prenatal protein malnutrition effects on the serotonergic system in the hippocampal formation: An immunocytochemical, ligand binding, and neurochemical study. Brain Research Bulletin 34(5): 507–5l8.

Borghese, C. M., Cordoba, N. E., Laino, C. H., Orsingher, O. A., Rubio, M. C., & Niselman, V. (1998). Lack of tolerance to the anxiolytic effect of diazepam and pentobarbital following chronic administration in perinatally undernourished rats. Brain Research Bulletin 46(3): 237–244.

Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2001). Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: Normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156(2–3): 234–258.

Braff, D. L., Grillon, C., & Geyer, M. A. (1992). Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 49(3): 206–215.

Brioni, J. D., Cordoba, N., & Orsingher, O. A. (1989). Decreased reactivity to the anticonflict effect of diazepam in perinatally undernourished rats. Behavioural Brain Research 34(1-2): 159–162.

Brioni, J. D., Keller, E. A., Levin, L. E., Cordoba, N., & Orsingher, O. A. (1986). Reactivity to amphetamine in perinatally undernourished rats: Behavioral and neurochemical correlates. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 24(3): 449–454.

Brioni, J. D. & Orsingher, O. A. (1987). Perinatal undernutrition alters hypothermic responses to different central agonists in recovered adult rats. Neuropharmacology 26(7A): 771–774.

Brioni, J. D. & Orsingher, O. A. (1988). Operant behavior and reactivity to the anticonflict effect of diazepam in perinatally undernourished rats. Physiology and Behavior 44(2): 193–198.

Brown, A. S. & Susser, E. S. (2008). Prenatal nutritional deficiency and risk of adult schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34(6): 1054–1063.

Brown, A. S., Susser, E. S., Lin, S. P., Neugebauer, R., & Gorman, J. M. (1995). Increased risk of affective disorders in males after second trimester prenatal exposure to the Dutch hunger winter of 1944–45. British Journal of Psychiatry 166(5): 601–606.

Brown, A. S., van Os, J., Driessens, C., Hoek, H. W., & Susser, E. S. (2000). Further evidence of relation between prenatal famine and major affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 157(2): 190–195.

Buckley, P. F., Miller, B. J., Lehrer, D. S., & Castle, D. J. (2009). Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35(2): 383–402.

Cadenhead, K. S., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (1993). Impaired startle prepulse inhibition and habituation in patients with schizotypal personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 150(12): 1862–1867.

Cannon, M., Byrne, M., Cotter, D., Sham, P., Larkin, C., & Ocallaghan, E. (1994). Further evidence for anomalies in the hand-prints of patients with schizophrenia—a study of secondary creases. Schizophrenia Research 13(2): 179–184.

Castanon-Cervantes, O. & Cintra, L. (2002). Circadian rhythms of occipital-cortex temperature and motor activity in young and old rats under chronic protein malnutrition. Nutritional Neuroscience 5(4): 279–286.

Chen, J. C., Turiak, G., Galler, J., & Volicer, L. (1995). Effect of prenatal malnutrition on release of monoamines from hippocampal slices. Life Sciences 57(16): 1467–1475.

Chrousos, G. P. (2009). Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews Endocrinology 5(7): 374–381.

Cintra, L., Aguilar, A., Granados, L., Galván, A., Kemper, T., DeBassio, W., Galler, J., Morgane, P., Durán, P., & Díaz-Cintra, S. (1997a). Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells in rats of four age groups. Hippocampus 7(22): 192–203.

Cintra, L., Duran, P., Guevara, M. A., Aguilar, A., & Castanon-Cervantes, O. (2002). Pre- and post-natal protein malnutrition alters the effect of rapid eye movements sleep-deprivation by the platform-technique upon the electrocorticogram of the circadian sleep-wake cycle and its frequency bands in the rat. Nutritional Neuroscience 5(2): 91–101.

Cintra, L., Granados, L., Aguilar, A., Kemper, T., DeBassio, W., Galler, J., Morgane, P., Durán, P., & Díaz-Cintra, S. (1997b). Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition on mossy fibers of the hippocampal formation in rats of four age groups. Hippocampus 7(2): 184–191.

Clancy, B., Darlington, R. B., & Finlay, B. L. (2001). Translating developmental time across mammalian species. Neuroscience 105(1): 7–17.

Cohrs, S. (2008). Sleep disturbances in patients with schizophrenia: Impact and effect of antipsychotics. CNS Drugs 22(11): 939–962.

Connolly, P. M., Maxwell, C. R., Kanes, S. J., Abel, T., Liang, Y., Tokarczyk, J., Bilker, W. B., Turetsky, B. I., Gur, R. E., & Siegel, S. J. (2003). Inhibition of auditory evoked potentials and prepulse inhibition of startle in DBA/2J and DBA/2Hsd inbred mouse substrains. Brain Research 992(1): 85–95.

Cooney, C. A., Dave, A. A., & Wolff, G. L. (2002). Maternal methyl supplements in mice affect epigenetic variation and DNA methylation of offspring. Journal of Nutrition 132(8 Suppl): 2393S–2400S.

Cordoba, N. E., Cuadra, G. R., Brioni, J. D., & Orsingher, O. A. (1992). Perinatal protein deprivation enhances the anticonflict effect measured after chronic ethanol administration in adult rats. Journal of Nutrition 122(7): 1536–1541.

Cordoba, N. E., Molina, J. C., Basso, A. M., & Orsingher, O. A. (1990). Perinatal undernutrition reduced ethanol intake preference in adult recovered rats. Physiology & Behavior 47(6): 1111–1116.

Cryan, J. F. & Mombereau, C. (2004). In search of a depressed mouse: Utility of models for studying depression-related behavior in genetically modified mice. Molecular Psychiatry 9(4): 326–357.

Damgaard, T., Larsen, D. B., Hansen, S. L., Grayson, B., Neill, J. C., & Plath, N. (2010). Positive modulation of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors reverses sub-chronic PCP-induced deficits in the novel object recognition task in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 207(1): 144–150.

Desbonnet, L., Waddington, J. L., & O’Tuathaigh, C. M. (2009a). Mice mutant for genes associated with schizophrenia: Common phenotype or distinct endophenotypes? Behavioural Brain Research 204(2): 258–273.

Desbonnet, L., Waddington, J. L., & O’Tuathaigh, C. M. (2009b). Mutant models for genes associated with schizophrenia. Biochemical Society Transactions 37(Pt 1): 308–312.

Díaz-Cintra, S., Cintra, L., Galván, A., Aguilar, A., Kemper, T., & Morgane, P. (1991). Effects of prenatal protein deprivation on postnatal development of granule cells in the fascia dentata. Journal of Comparative Neurology 310(3): 356–364.

Díaz-Cintra, S., García-Ruiz, M., Corkidi, G., & Cintra, L. (1994). Effects of prenatal malnutrition and postnatal nutritional rehabilitation on CA3 hippocampal pyramidal cells in rats of four ages. Brain Research 662(1–2): 117–126.

Díaz-Cintra, S., González-Maciel, A., Morales, M. A., Aguilar, A., Cintra, L., & Prado-Alcalá, R. A. (2007). Protein malnutrition differentially alters the number of glutamic acid decarboxylase-67 interneurons in dentate gyrus and CA1–3 subfields of the dorsal hippocampus. Experimental Neurology 208(1): 47–53.

Di Matteo, V., Di Giovanni, G., Pierucci, M., & Esposito, E. (2008). Serotonin control of central dopaminergic function: Focus on in vivo microdialysis studies. Progress in Brain Research 172: 7–44.

Dulawa, S. C., Scearce-Levie, K. A., Hen, R., & Geyer, M. A. (2000). Serotonin releasers increase prepulse inhibition in serotonin 1B knockout mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 149(3): 306–312.

Duran, P., Cintra, L., Galler, J. R., & Tonkiss, J. (2005). Prenatal protein malnutrition induces a phase shift advance of the spontaneous locomotor rhythm and alters the rest/activity ratio in adult rats. Nutritional Neuroscience 8(3): 167–172.

Duran, P., Galler, J. R., Cintra, L., & Tonkiss, J. (2006). Prenatal malnutrition and sleep states in adult rats: Effects of restraint stress. Physiology and Behavior 89(2): 156–163.

Dzirasa, K., Ribeiro, S., Costa, R., Santos, L. M., Lin, S. C., Grosmark, A., Sotnikova, T. D., Gainetdinov, R. R., Caron, M. G., & Nicolelis, M. A. (2006). Dopaminergic control of sleep-wake states. Journal of Neuroscience 26(41): 10577–10589.

Ellenbroek, B. A. & Cools, A. R. (2002). Apomorphine susceptibility and animal models for psychopathology: Genes and environment. Behavior Genetics 32(5): 349–361.

Etherton, M. R., Blaiss, C. A., Powell, C. M., & Sudhof, T. C. (2009). Mouse neurexin-1 alpha deletion causes correlated electrophysiological and behavioral changes consistent with cognitive impairments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 106(42): 17998–18003.

Farrow, T. F., Hunter, M. D., Haque, R., & Spence, S. A. (2006). Modafinil and unconstrained motor activity in schizophrenia: Double-blind crossover placebo-controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 189: 461–462.

Fatemi, S. H. & Folsom, T. D. (2009). The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, revisited. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35(3): 528–548.

Fenton, W. S., Stover, E. L., & Insel, T. R. (2003). Breaking the log-jam in treatment development for cognition in schizophrenia: NIMH perspective. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 169(3–4): 365–366.

Fiacco, T. A., Rosene, D. L., Galler, J. R., & Blatt, G. J. (2003). Increased density of hippocampal kainate receptors but normal density of NMDA and AMPA receptors in a rat model of prenatal protein malnutrition. Journal of Comparative Neurology 456(4): 350–360.

Francolin-Silva, A. L. & Almeida, S. S. (2004). The interaction of housing condition and acute immobilization stress on the elevated plus-maze behaviors of protein-malnourished rats. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research 37(7): 1035–1042.

Francolin-Silva, A. L., Brandao, M. L., & Almeida, S. S. (2007). Early postnatal protein malnutrition causes resistance to the anxiolytic effects of diazepam as assessed by the fear-potentiated startle test. Nutritional Neuroscience 10(1–2): 23–29.

Francolin-Silva, A. L., da Silva Hernandes, A., Fukuda, M. T., Valadares, C. T., & Almeida, S. S. (2006). Anxiolytic-like effects of short-term postnatal protein malnutrition in the elevated plus-maze test. Behavioural Brain Research 173(2): 310–314.

Frankova, S. (1973). Effect of protein-calorie malnutrition on the development of social behavior in rats. Developmental Psychobiology 6(1) 33–43.

Galler, J. R., Tonkiss, J., & Maldonado-Irizarry, C. S. (1994). Prenatal protein malnutrition and home orientation in the rat. Physiology and Behavior 55(6): 993–996.

Gallo, P. V., Werboff, J., & Knox, K. (1984). Development of home orientation in offspring of protein-restricted cats. Developmental Psychobiology 17(5): 437–449.

Garzya, V., Forbes, I. T., Gribble, A. D., Hadley, M. S., Lightfoot, A. P., Payne, A. H., Smith, A. B., Douglas, S. E., Cooper, D. G., Stansfield, I. G., et al. (2007). Studies towards the identification of a new generation of atypical antipsychotic agents. Bioorganic and Medicinal Chemistry Letters 17(2): 400–405.

Geyer, M. A. & Braff, D. L. (1982). Habituation of the Blink reflex in normals and schizophrenic patients. Psychophysiology 19(1): 1–6.

Geyer, M. A., Krebs-Thomson, K., Braff, D. L., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2001). Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: A decade in review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156(2–3): 117–154.

Geyer, M. A. & Markou, A. (1995). Animal models of psychiatric disorders. In F. Bloom & D. Kupfer (eds.), Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress (pp. 787–798). New York: Raven.

Geyer, M. A. & Vollenweider, F. X. (2008). Serotonin research: Contributions to understanding psychoses. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 29(9): 445–453.

Goodlett, C. R., Valentino, M. L., Morgane, P. J., & Resnick, O. (1986). Spatial cue utilization in chronically malnourished rats: Task-specific learning deficits. Developmental Psychobiology 19(1): 1–15.

Goodlett, C. R., Valentino, M. L., Resnick, O., & Morgane, P. J. (1985). Altered development of responsiveness to clonidine in severely malnourished rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 23(4): 567–572.

Gottesman, I. I. & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry 160(4): 636–645.

Green, M. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., Gold, J. M., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J., Essock, S., Fenton, W. S., Frese, F., Goldberg, T. E., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2004). Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia: The NIMH-MATRICS conference to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biological Psychiatry 56(5): 301–307.

Gressens, P., Muaku, S. M., Besse, L., Nsegbe, E., Gallego, J., Delpech, B., Gaultier, C., Evrard, P., Ketelslegers, J. M., & Maiter, D. (1997). Maternal protein restriction early in rat pregnancy alters brain development in the progeny. Brain Research: Developmental Brain Research 103(1): 21–35.

Hall, R. D. (1983). Is hippocampal function in the adult rat impaired by early protein or protein-calorie deficiencies? Developmental Psychobiology 16(5): 395–411.

Hall, R. D., Leahy, J. P., & Robertson, W. M. (1983). Hyposensitivity to serotonergic stimulation in protein malnourished rats. Physiology and Behavior 31(2): 187–195.

Heijmans, B. T., Tobi, E. W., Stein, A. D., Putter, H., Blauw, G. J., Susser, E. S., Slagboom, P. E., & Lumey, L. H. (2008). Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 105(44): 17046–17049.

Hernandes, A. S. & Almeida, S. S. (2003). Postnatal protein malnutrition affects inhibitory avoidance and risk assessment behaviors in two models of anxiety in rats. Nutritional Neuroscience 6(4): 213–219.

Hoek, H. W., Brown, A. S., & Susser, E. (1998). The Dutch Famine and schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 33(8): 373–379.

Howes, O. D., Egerton, A., Allan, V., McGuire, P., Stokes, P., & Kapur, S. (2009). Mechanisms underlying psychosis and antipsychotic treatment response in schizophrenia: Insights from PET and SPECT imaging. Current Pharmaceutical Design 15(22): 2550–2559.

Hulshoff Pol, H. E. & Kahn, R. S. (2008). What happens after the first episode? A review of progressive brain changes in chronically ill patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34(2): 354–366.

Javitt, D. C. (2006). Is the glycine site half saturated or half unsaturated? Effects of glutamatergic drugs in schizophrenia patients. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 19(2): 151–157.

Javitt, D. C., Spencer, K. M., Thaker, G. K., Winterer, G., & Hajos, M. (2008). Neurophysiological biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 7(1): 68–83.

Jones, D. N. C., Gartlon, J. E., Minassian, A., Perry, W., & Geyer, M. A. (2008). Developing new drugs for schizophrenia: From animals to the clinic. In R. McArthur & F. Borsini (eds.), Animal and Translational Models for CNS Drug Discovery (vol. 1) (pp. 199–261). New York: Academic Press.

Joyce, J. N., Lexow, N., Bird, E., & Winokur, A. (1988). Organization of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in human striatum: Receptor autoradiographic studies in Huntington’s disease and schizophrenia. Synapse 2(5): 546–557.

Kehoe, P., Mallinson, K., Bronzino, J., & McCormick, C. M. (2001). Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition and neonatal stress on CNS responsiveness. Brain Research: Developmental Brain Research 132(1): 23–31.

Kellendonk, C., Simpson, E. H., & Kandel, E. R. (2009). Modeling cognitive endophenotypes of schizophrenia in mice. Trends in Neuroscience 32(6): 347–358.

Keller, E. A., Cancela, L. M., Molina, V. A., & Orsingher, O. A. (1994). Lack of adaptive changes in 5-HT sites in perinatally undernourished rats after chronic stress: Opioid influence. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 47(4): 789–793.

Keller, E. A., Molina, V. A., & Orsingher, O. A. (1990). Lack of neuronal adaptive changes following chronic treatments in perinatally undernourished rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 37(4): 675–678.

Laino, C. H., Cordoba, N. E., & Orsingher, O. A. (1993). Perinatally protein-deprived rats and reactivity to anxiolytic drugs in the plus-maze test: An animal model for screening antipanic agents? Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 46(1): 89–94.

Large, C. H. (2007). Do NMDA receptor antagonist models of schizophrenia predict the clinical efficacy of antipsychotic drugs? Journal of Psychopharmacology 21(3): 283–301.

Leahy, J. P., Stern, W. C., Resnick, O., & Morgane, P. J. (1978). A neuropharmacological analysis of central nervous system catecholamine systems in development protein malnutrition. Developmental Psychobiology 11(4): 361–370.

Le Pen, G., Gaudet, L., Mortas, P., Mory, R., & Moreau, J. L. (2002) Deficits in reward sensitivity in a neurodevelopmental rat model of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 161(4): 434–441.

Li, L., Du, Y., Li, N., Wu, X., & Wu, Y. (2009). Top-down modulation of prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in humans and rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33(8): 1157–1167.

Lillycrop, K. A., Phillips, E. S., Jackson, A. A., Hanson, M. A., & Burdge, G. C. (2005). Dietary protein restriction of pregnant rats induces and folic acid supplementation prevents epigenetic modification of hepatic gene expression in the offspring. Journal of Nutrition 135(6): 1382–1386.

Lillycrop, K. A., Phillips, E. S., Torrens, C., Hanson, M. A., Jackson, A. A., & Burdge, G. C. (2008). Feeding pregnant rats a protein-restricted diet persistently alters the methylation of specific cytosines in the hepatic PPAR alpha promoter of the offspring. British Journal of Nutrition 100(2): 278–282.

Lipska, B. K., Swerdlow, N. R., Geyer, M. A., Jaskiw, G. E., Braff, D. L., & Weinberger, D. R. (1995). Neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage in rats causes post-pubertal changes in prepulse inhibition of startle and its disruption by apomorphine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 122(1): 35–43.

Lister, J. P., Blatt, G. J., DeBassio, W. A., Kemper, T. L., Tonkiss, J., Galler, J. R., & Rosene, D. L. (2005). Effect of prenatal protein malnutrition on numbers of neurons in the principal cell layers of the adult rat hippocampal formation. Hippocampus 15(3): 393–403.

Lodge, D. J. & Grace, A. A. (2008) Hippocampal dysfunction and disruption of dopamine system regulation in an animal model of schizophrenia. Neurotoxicity Research 14(2–3): 97–104.

Lopes de Souza, S., Orozco-Solis, R., Grit, I., Manhães de Castro, R., & Bolaños-Jiménez, F. (2008). Perinatal protein restriction reduces the inhibitory action of serotonin on food intake. European Journal of Neuroscience 27(6): 1400–1408.

Marichich, E. S., Molina, V. A., & Orsingher, O. A. (1979). Persistent changes in central catecholaminergic system after recovery of perinatally undernourished rats. Journal of Nutrition 109(6): 1045–1050.

Meyer, U. & Feldon, J. (2009). Epidemiology-driven neurodevelopmental animal models of schizophrenia. Progress in Neurobiology 90(3): 285–326.

Mokler, D. J., Galle, J. R., & Morgane, P. J. (2003). Modulation of 5-HT release in the hippocampus of 30-day-old rats exposed in utero to protein malnutrition. Brain Research: Developmental Brain Research 142(2): 203–208.

Mokler, D. J., Torres, O. I., Galler, J. R., & Morgane, P. J. (2007). Stress-induced changes in extracellular dopamine and serotonin in the medial prefrontal cortex and dorsal hippocampus of prenatally malnourished rats. Brain Research 1148: 226–233.

Morgane, P. J., Austin-LaFrance, R., Bronzino, J., Tonkiss, J., Diaz-Cintra, S., Cintra, L., Kemper, T., & Galler, J. R. (1993). Prenatal malnutrition and development of the brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 17(1): 91–128.

Niculescu, M. D. & Zeisel, S. H. (2002). Diet, methyl donors and DNA methylation: Interactions between dietary folate, methionine and choline. Journal of Nutrition 132(8 Suppl): 2333S–2335S.

Palmer, A. A., Brown, A. S., Keegan, D., Siska, L. D., Susser, E., Rotrosen, J., & Butler, P. D. (2008). Prenatal protein deprivation alters dopamine-mediated behaviors and dopaminergic and glutamatergic receptor binding. Brain Research 1237: 62–74.

Palmer, A. A., Printz, D. J., Butler, P. D., Dulawa, S. C., & Printz, M. P. (2004). Prenatal protein deprivation in rats induces changes in prepulse inhibition and NMDA receptor binding. Brain Research 996(2): 193–201.

Patil, S. T., Zhang, L., Martenyi, F., Lowe, S. L., Jackson, K. A., Andreev, B. V., Avedisova, A. S., Bardenstein, L. M., Gurovich, I. Y., Morozova, M. A., et al. (2007). Activation of mGlu2/3 receptors as a new approach to treat schizophrenia: A randomized phase 2 clinical trial. Nature Medicine 13(9): 1102–1107.

Patterson, P. H. (2009). Immune involvement in schizophrenia and autism: Etiology, pathology and animal models. Behavioural Brain Research 204(2): 313–321.

Paz, R. D., Tardito, S., Atzori, M., & Tseng, K. Y. (2008). Glutamatergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: From basic neuroscience to clinical psychopharmacology. European Neuropsychopharmacology 18(11): 773–786.

Perry, W., Minassian, A., Feifel, D., & Braff, D. L. (2001). Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biological Psychiatry 50(6): 418–424.

Powell, S. B., Zhou, X., & Geyer, M. A. (2009). Prepulse inhibition and genetic mouse models of schizophrenia. Behavioural Brain Research 204(2): 282–294.

Prut, L., & Belzung, C. (2003). The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: A review. European Journal of Pharmacology 463(1–3): 3–33.

Ranade, S. C., Rose, A., Rao, M., Gallego, J., Gressens, P., & Mani, S. (2008). Different types of nutritional deficiencies affect different domains of spatial memory function checked in a radial arm maze. Neuroscience 152(4): 859–866.

Resnick, O. & Morgane, P. J. (1984). Ontogeny of the levels of serotonin in various parts of the brain in severely protein malnourished rats. Brain Research 303(1): 163–170.

Rodgers, R. J. & Johnson, N. J. (1995). Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and ethological measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 52(2): 297–303.

Rojas, P., Montes, S., Serrano-Garcia, N., & Rojas-Castaneda, J. (2009). Effect of EGb761 supplementation on the content of copper in mouse brain in an animal model of Parkinson’s disease. Nutrition 25(4): 482–485.

Sams-Dodd, F. (1999). Phencyclidine in the social interaction test: An animal model of schizophrenia with face and predictive validity. Reviews in Neurosciences 10(1): 59–90.

Santucci, L. B., Daud, M. M., Almeida, S. S., & de Oliveira, L. M. (1994). Effects of early protein malnutrition and environmental stimulation upon the reactivity to diazepam in two animal models of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 49(2): 393–398.

Schumacher, A., Kapranov, P., Kaminsky, Z., Flanagan, J., Assadzadeh, A., Yau, P., Virtanen, C., Winegarden, N., Cheng, J., Gingeras, T., et al. (2006). Microarray-based DNA methylation profiling: Technology and applications. Nucleic Acids Research 34(2): 528–542.

Schweigert, I. D., de Oliveira, D. L., Scheibel, F., da Costa, F., Wofchuk, S. T., Souza, D. O., & Perry, M. L. (2005). Gestational and postnatal malnutrition affects sensitivity of young rats to picrotoxin and quinolinic acid and uptake of GABA by cortical and hippocampal slices. Brain Research: Developmental Brain Research 154(2): 177–185.

Seeman, P., Bzowej, N. H., Guan, H. C., Bergeron, C., Reynolds, G. P., Bird, E. D., Riederer, P., Jellinger, K., & Tourtellotte, W. W. (1987). Human brain D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s, and Huntington’s diseases. Neuropsychopharmacology 1(1): 5–15.

Shanahan, N. A., Holick Pierz, K. A., Masten, V. L., Waeber, C., Ansorge, M., Gingrich, J. A., Geyer, M. A., Hen, R., & Dulawa, S. C. (2009). Chronic reductions in serotonin transporter function prevent 5-HT1B-induced behavioral effects in mice. Biological Psychiatry 65(5): 401–408.

Shultz, P. L., Galler, J. R., & Tonkiss, J. (1999). Prenatal protein restriction increases sensitization to cocaine-induced stereotypy. Behavioural Pharmacology 10(4): 379–387.

Shultz, P. L., Galler, J. R., & Tonkiss, J. (2002). Prenatal protein malnutrition enhances stimulus control by CDP, but not a CDP/THIP combination in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 73(4): 759–767.

Singh, S. M., Murphy, B., & O’Reilly, R. L. (2003). Involvement of gene-diet/drug interaction in DNA methylation and its contribution to complex diseases: From cancer to schizophrenia. Clinical Genetics 64(6) 451–460.

Smart, J. L., Dobbing, J., Adlard, B. P., Lynch, A., & Sands, J. (1973). Vulnerability of developing brain: Relative effects of growth restriction during the fetal and suckling periods on behavior and brain composition of adult rats. Journal of Nutrition 103(9): 1327–1338.

St Clair, D., Xu, M., Wang, P., Yu, Y., Fang, Y., Zhang, F., Zheng, X., Gu, N., Feng, G., Sham, P., et al. (2005). Rates of adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to the Chinese Famine of 1959–1961. Journal of the American Medical Association 294: 557–562.

Steiger, J. L., Alexander, M. J., Galler, J. R., Farb, D. H., & Russek, S. J. (2003). Effects of prenatal malnutrition on GABAA receptor alpha1, alpha3 and beta2 mRNA levels. Neuroreport 14(13): 1731–1735.

Steiger, J. L., Galler, J. R., Farb, D. H., & Russek, S. J. (2002). Prenatal protein malnutrition reduces beta(2), beta(3) and gamma(2L) GABA(A) receptor subunit mRNAs in the adult septum. European Journal of Pharmacology 446(1–3): 201–202.

Stone, J. M., Morrison, P. D., & Pilowsky, L. S. (2007). Glutamate and dopamine dysregulation in schizophrenia—a synthesis and selective review. Journal of Psychopharmacology 21(4): 440–452.

Susser, E., Neugebauer, R., Hoek, H., Brown, A., Lin, S., Labovitz, D., & Gorman, J. (1996). Schizophrenia after prenatal famine: Further evidence. Archives of General Psychiatry 53 : 25–31.

Susser, E. S. & Lin, S. P. (1992). Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch Hunger Winter of 1944–1945. Archives of General Psychiatry 49(12): 983–988.

Swerdlow, N. R., Braff, D. L., & Geyer, M. A. (1990). GABAergic projection from nucleus accumbens to ventral pallidum mediates dopamine-induced sensorimotor gating deficits of acoustic startle in rats. Brain Research 532(1–2): 146–150.

Swerdlow, N. R., Braff, D. L., Taaid, N., & Geyer, M. A. (1994). Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 51(2): 139–154.

Swerdlow, N. R., Weber, M., Qu, Y., Light, G. A., & Braff, D. L. (2008). Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199(3): 331–388.

Toda, M. & Abi-Dargham, A. (2007). Dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: Making sense of it all. Current Psychiatry Reports 9(4): 329–336.

Tonkiss, J. & Galler, J. (2007). Prenatal malnutrition alters diazepam-mediated suppression of ultrasonic vocalizations in an age dependent manner. Behavioural Brain Research 182(2): 337–343.

Tonkiss, J., Galler, J., Morgane, P. J., Bronzino, J. D., & Austin-LaFrance, R. J. (1993). Prenatal protein malnutrition and postnatal brain function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 678: 215–227.

Tonkiss, J. & Galler, J. R. (1990). Prenatal protein malnutrition and working memory performance in adult rats. Behavioural Brain Research 40(2): 95–107.

Tonkiss, J., Galler, J. R., Formica, R. N., Shukitt-Hale, B., & Timm, R. R. (1990a). Fetal protein malnutrition impairs acquisition of a DRL task in adult rats. Physiology and Behavior 48(1): 73–77.

Tonkiss, J., Harrison, R. H., & Galler, J. R. (1996). Differential effects of prenatal protein malnutrition and prenatal cocaine on a test of homing behavior in rat pups. Physiology and Behavior 60(3): 1013–1018.

Tonkiss, J., Shukitt-Hale, B., Formica, R. N., Rocco, F. J., & Galler, J. R. (1990b). Prenatal protein malnutrition alters response to reward in adult rats. Physiology and Behavior 48(5): 675–680.

Tonkiss, J., Shultz, P., & Galler, J. R. (1994). An analysis of spatial navigation in prenatally protein malnourished rats. Physiology and Behavior 55(2): 217–224.

Tonkiss, J., Shultz, P. L., Shumsky, J. S., Fiacco, T. T., Vincitore, M., Rosene, D. L., & Galler, J. R. (2000). Chlordiazepoxide-induced spatial learning deficits: Dose-dependent differences following prenatal malnutrition. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 65(1): 105–116.

Tonkiss, J. & Smart, J. L. 1983). Interactive effects of genotype and early life undernutrition on the development of behavior in rats. Developmental Psychobiology 16(4): 287–301

Torres, G., Meeder, B. A., Hallas, B. H., Gross, K. W., & Horowitz, J. M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for reduced social interactions in Chakragati mutants modeling certain symptoms of schizophrenia. Brain Research 1046(1–2): 180–186.

Trzctnska, M., Tonkiss, J., & Galler, J. R. (1999). Influence of prenatal protein malnutrition on behavioral reactivity to stress in adult rats. Stress 3(1): 71–83.

Turetsky, B. I., Calkins, M. E., Light, G. A., Olincy, A., Radant, A. D., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2007). Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: The viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33(1): 69–94.

Ueki, A., Goto, K., Sato, N., Iso, H., & Morita, Y. (2006). Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle response in mild cognitive impairment and mild dementia of Alzheimer type. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 60(1): 55–62.

Umbricht, D. & Krljes, S. (2005). Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 76(1): 1–23.

Umbricht, D., Vollenweider, F. X., Schmid, L., Grubel, C., Skrabo, A., Huber, T., & Koller, R. (2003). Effects of the 5-HT2A agonist psilocybin on mismatch negativity generation and AX-continuous performance task: Implications for the neuropharmacology of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 28(1): 170–181.

Umbricht, D., Vyssotki, D., Latanov, A., Nitsch, R., & Lipp, H. P. (2005). Deviance-related electrophysiological activity in mice: Is there mismatch negativity in mice? Clinical Neurophysiology 116(2): 353–363.

Vachtenheim, J., Horakova, I., & Novotna, H. (1994). Hypomethylation of CCGG sites in the 3’ region of H-ras protooncogene is frequent and is associated with H-ras allele loss in non-small cell lung cancer. Cancer Research 54(5): 1145–1148.

Valdomero, A., Bussolino, D. F., Orsingher, O. A., & Cuadra, G. R. (2006). Perinatal protein malnutrition enhances rewarding cocaine properties in adult rats. Neuroscience 137(1): 221–229.

Valdomero, A., Isoardi, N. A., Orsingher, O. A., & Cuadra, G. R. (2005). Pharmacological reactivity to cocaine in adult rats undernourished at perinatal age: Behavioral and neurochemical correlates. Neuropharmacology 48(4): 538–546.

Valdomero, A., Velazquez, E. E., de Olmos, S., de Olmos, J. S., Orsingher, O. A., & Cuadra, G. R. (2007). Increased rewarding properties of morphine in perinatally protein-malnourished rats. Neuroscience 150(2): 449–458.

van den Buuse, M., Garner, B., Gogos, A., & Kusljic, S. (2005). Importance of animal models in schizophrenia research. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 39(7): 550–557.

Vollenweider, F. X., Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen, M. F., Babler, A., Vogel, H., & Hell, D. (1998). Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. Neuroreport 9(17): 3897–3902.

Wainfan, E., Dizik, M., Stender, M., & Christman, J. K. (1989). Rapid appearance of hypo-methylated DNA in livers of rats fed cancer-promoting, methyl-deficient diets. Cancer Research 49(15): 4094–4097.

Watkins, A. J., Ursell, E., Panton, R., Papenbrock, T., Hollis, L., Cunningham, C., Wilkins, A., Perry, V. H., Sheth, B., Kwong, W. Y., et al. (2008a). Adaptive responses by mouse early embryos to maternal diet protect fetal growth but predispose to adult onset disease. Biological Reproduction 78(2): 299–306.

Watkins, A. J., Wilkins, A., Cunningham, C., Perry, V. H., Seet, M. J., Osmond, C., Eckert, J. J., Torrens, C., Cagampang, F. R., Cleal, J., et al. (2008b). Low protein diet fed exclusively during mouse oocyte maturation leads to behavioural and cardiovascular abnormalities in offspring. Journal of Physiology 586(8): 2231–2244.

Weiner, I. & Arad, M. (2009). Using the pharmacology of latent inhibition to model domains of pathology in schizophrenia and their treatment. Behavioural Brain Research 204(2): 369–386.

Weiss, I. C. & Feldon, J. (2001). Environmental animal models for sensorimotor gating deficiencies in schizophrenia: A review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156(2–3): 305–326.

Whatson, T. S., Smart, J. L., & Dobbing, J. (1975). Dominance relationships among previously undernourished and well fed male rats. Physiology and Behavior 14(04): 425–429.

Whatson, T. S., Smart, J. L., & Dobbing, J. (1976). Undernutrition in early life: Lasting effects on activity and social behavior of male and female rats. Developmental Psychobiology 9(6): 529–538.

Wiley, J. L. & Compton, A. D. (2004). Progressive ratio performance following challenge with antipsychotics, amphetamine, or NMDA antagonists in adult rats treated perinatally with phencyclidine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 177(1–2): 170–177.

Wolff, G. L., Kodell, R. L., Moore, S. R., & Cooney, C. A. (1998). Maternal epigenetics and methyl supplements affect agouti gene expression in Avy/a mice. FASEB Journal 12(11):949–957.

Xu, M., Wen-Sheng, S., Liu, B., Feng, G., Yu, L., Yang, L., He, G., Sham, P., Susser, E., St Clair, D., et al. (2009). Prenatal malnutrition and adult schizophrenia: Further evidence from the 1959–1961 famine. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35(3): 568–576.

Young, J. W., Minassian, A., Paulus, M. P., Geyer, M. A., & Perry, W. (2007). A reverse-translational approach to bipolar disorder: Rodent and human studies in the Behavioral Pattern Monitor. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews31(6): 882–896.