CHAPTER FOUR
Overview of General Methods
In 1975, researchers Fergus I. M. Craik and Endel Tulving at the University of Toronto in Ontario, Canada, designed a series of ten experiments to explore the levels of processing within the human memory. They presented their participants with a series of sixty words, and asked them one of three questions about each word.
One of the questions asked strictly for visual processing of the word: “Is the word in capital letters or small letters?” Another asked them to consider the sound of the word: “Does the word rhyme with?” The third question required the participant to think a little more deeply about the word: “Does the word fit into this sentence?”
After they were quizzed in this manner about the original sixty words, the researchers handed each participant a list of 180 words to consider. The first sixty words were mixed into this much longer list. Their task: Pick out the words that had been in the original list of sixty.
What do you think happened? If you guessed that the participants remembered many more of the words about which they’d been asked the more meaningful question—if the word fit into a specific sentence—then you are correct. The deeper question, involving the meaning of the word, required a process that scientists call elaboration rehearsal, making the participant think about what the word meant and how it would be used, rather than simply about what it looked like or how it sounded.
This breakthrough research was the first to explain the workings of memory in a less structural, more process-oriented fashion. Where prevailing theories about memory at the time focused on the separation of short-term and long-term memory and the requirements for a memory to move from one to the other, this new study showed that the way the mind actually processes the memory has a great deal to do with its retention.
Craik and Tulving saw the memory’s ability to process information in three ways: structurally, phonemically, and semantically.
This study opened up new avenues for scientific exploration of the formation of memories. At the same time, it pointed the way to new methods for improving the brain’s ability to retain information. No longer a mechanical concept, memory retention moved into the world of context and semantics, creating mental hooks that linked one kind of information to another for stronger memories and longer retention.
Engaging the Whole Mind
Students and adults are expected to learn a great deal of information over the course of a lifetime. A new job can require long hours of training or retraining, a quest for a law degree can mean intensive memorization, and the process of earning a medical degree means acquiring copious amounts of facts about the human body and its detailed systems.
If you’ve ever sat through a dull meeting and realized you retained none of the information once it was over, you know that it’s important to engage your mind—not just a little bit, but in total—to take in information and keep hold of it after the event has passed.
As you begin your exploration of ways to improve memory, here are some of the ground rules that will be useful no matter which method you embrace.
If you’re taking a lecture course or sitting through a meeting that does not offer much opportunity to ramp up your participation or interaction with the subject matter, you can create opportunities for active learning on your own.
When you took the memory test in Chapter Three, did you say the words or the numbers aloud as you worked to commit them to memory? This kind of repetition, including the use of another sense besides sight, makes a stronger impression on your memory and helps you keep the information at the top of your mind. Further rehearsal—more repetition over a longer time period—helps you retain it beyond the few minutes in which you needed it.
Think, for example, about all of the song lyrics you may know, even if you have never memorized a poem without music in your life. The process of singing along with the recording, hearing the song over and over, and connecting the music and lyrics together turns that lyric into a long-term memory. Some people remember song lyrics they learned in nursery school well into old age, even if they do not hear that song again in adulthood. (You’ll find more on songs and rhyming as a memory technique in Chapter Nine.)
Scaffolding
The memory improvement techniques discussed in the chapters that follow all hinge on a basic premise: linking new memories with things you already know. Experts call this scaffolding, a process of building on prior knowledge to form bonds between old memories and new ones.
Have you ever wondered when babies begin to remember? Scientists have determined that children do not form explicit memories—a full account of a specific event, for example—until they reach about three years old. There’s no question that babies begin to remember things much earlier, however. In fact, researchers have found that newborn infants react to their mother’s voice within minutes of birth, because the infant heard that voice continually while in the womb.
The ability to remember is established even before birth, though the meaning and context of such memories takes years to develop. Babies’ minds learn to create patterns of sensory impressions that become specific people, items, or experiences, based on repeated attempts that teach them basic principles. If you’ve ever watched a baby drop a toy from the high chair’s tray to the floor over and over again, you are seeing a memory being constructed at that very moment. The baby lets go, the toy falls . . . and Mommy picks it up and gives it back. These events become linked in the baby’s brain, and learning takes place. The baby will not remember the specifics of the day, time, location, or even the toy itself—what researchers call the long-term explicit memory—but he will remember that Mommy picks up toys that drop.
Once babies and young children build a foundation of these basic memories from a very early point in their lives, they pile on new memories at a rapid pace. Knowing one thing—that Mommy will pick up the fallen toy—leads to learning about a related thing: Mommy also will pick up food that falls. Using this information as if it were scaffolding, the baby soon learns that Mommy will lift him out of the high chair when he wants to get down. All of these bits of knowledge center on activity in the high chair, the familiar point of reference that forms the basis for learning.
Building the scaffolding from an established memory to a new one can take conscious effort, especially when you are working to take in a lot of new information at once. Techniques like association (Chapter Five), visualization (Chapter Six), and the method of loci (Chapter Eight) are based on creating linkages between what you already know and what you want to learn.
Organize to Remember
Your desk may be cluttered, your garage may need a cleaning, and your attic or basement may cry out for a good purge, but your mind requires a certain amount of order to be able to form lasting memories. Your ability to organize information in meaningful ways can go a long way in helping you retain and recall memories when you need them.
What if you had to memorize a string of forty-five letters, all strung together at random? Such a list might look like this:
alskfupcwelkjowirutjnclcjdowhvbgkdnwkdycogneo
How hard would this be to memorize? For most people, it would be virtually impossible to accomplish.
But what about if you had to memorize this forty-five-letter sentence:
Marley and Josiah could not wait to play in the new snow.
Suddenly it’s not hard at all to memorize forty-five letters. A collection of letters makes sense when it’s broken up into words, each of which has a meaning you can recall instantly.
A number of memory improvement techniques use the basic concept of order to help you remember information.
All of these methods employ mnemonics, devices that are used to improve memory. A mnemonic is a way of reconstructing the content of the new information, with the intention of tying it to knowledge you already have.
The human mind remembers some kinds of information more easily than others:
Mnemonics can take many different forms, but if the one you use links to any of these specific kinds of information, you will create an association that may still be with you in the last days of your life.