Figure 1.1 |
Cloisonné vase with sunfish, made using gold wire and enamels. 2 |
Figure 1.2 |
Female cardinal evaluating a male in sunlight against natural foliage. 4 |
Figure 1.3 |
Fossilized compound eyes of two trilobite species. 5 |
Figure 1.4 |
A view of visual evolution as a punctuate process, advancing through a series of steps that represent significant leaps in visual capabilities. 6 |
Figure 1.5 |
Evolution of modern opsins. 7 |
Figure 2.1 |
The elephant hawkmoth (Deilephila elpenor) as viewed during late twilight and on a moonless night, showing the extreme chromatic changes that can occur within 30 minutes. 11 |
Figure 2.2 |
The electromagnetic spectrum. 12 |
Figure 2.3 |
Daylight spectrum binned by equal wavelength intervals and by equal frequency intervals. 14 |
Figure 2.4 |
The three most common light measurements: vector irradiance, radiance, and scalar irradiance. 15 |
Figure 2.5 |
Solar irradiance outside the atmosphere and at the Earth’s surface. 16 |
Figure 2.6 |
Fraction of irradiance at Earth’s surface near noon due to skylight alone. 17 |
Figure 2.7 |
Downwelling vector irradiance and scalar irradiance (at 560 nm) as a function of solar elevation. 18 |
Figure 2.8 |
Illuminance due to moon and sun at various times of day. 19 |
Figure 2.9 |
Irradiance on a North Carolina beach when sun is 10.6° below the horizon (late nautical twilight). 19 |
Figure 2.10 |
Landscape photographed under full moonlight and spectral reflectance of the moon. 20 |
Figure 2.11 |
Lunar irradiance (normalized to 1 for a full moon) and fraction of moon illuminated as a function of degrees from full moon. 21 |
Figure 2.12 |
Landscape under starlight and reconstructed downwelling irradiance spectrum of moonless night sky. 22 |
Figure 2.13 |
A green aurora combined with an unusually intense red aurora. 23 |
Figure 2.14 |
Light pollution in New York City and downwelling irradiance spectrum dominated by light pollution on a cloudy night at Jamaica Pond in Boston, MA. 24 |
Figure 2.15 |
Human-based chromaticities (i.e., perceived color) of daylight, sunset, twilight, and nocturnal irradiances. 25 |
Figure 2.16 |
Spectra of downwelling irradiance in a forest near Baltimore, MD. 26 |
Figure 2.17 |
The first two principal components (statistical measures of major sources of variation) of the spectra shown in figure 2.16. 26 |
Figure 2.18 |
The absorption and scattering coefficients of the clearest natural waters, along with their sum. 27 |
Figure 2.19 |
Downwelling irradiance in the equatorial Pacific modeled from vertical profiles of absorption, scattering, and chlorophyll concentration. 28 |
Figure 2.20 |
Radiance as a function of viewing angle and depth in equatorial Pacific waters with the sun 45° above the horizon. 30 |
Figure 2.21 |
Peak wavelength versus emission width for the light emissions from deep-sea benthic and mesopelagic species. 33 |
Figure 2.22 |
Examples of the various uses of bioluminescence. 34 |
Figure 2.23 |
Ambient light images of a deep-sea volcanic vent at nine visible and near-infrared wavelengths imaged by the ALISS camera system. Spectrum of emitted light at four locations within vent compared to the spectrum of a blackbody radiator at the vent temperature. 35 |
Figure 3.1 |
Structures of vertebrate and invertebrate visual pigments viewed from the plane of the plasma membranes of photoreceptor cells. 38 |
Figure 3.2 |
Structure of retinal, the most common chromophore found in visual pigments. 39 |
Figure 3.3 |
Typical absorption spectra of visual pigment molecules normalized to the same peak value. 40 |
Figure 3.4 |
The four types of chromophores known to be used in visual pigments. 41 |
Figure 3.5 |
Spectral effects on a visual pigment of changing from a retinal chromophore to a 3-dehydroretinal chromophore and how the wavelength of maximum absorption changes when the same opsin is bound to an A1 or an A2 chromophore. 42 |
Figure 3.6 |
Schematic illustration of dichroism in visual pigments. 43 |
Figure 3.7 |
Seemingly unimportant changes in absorbance can produce major effects on how a photoreceptor cell actually absorbs light. 45 |
Figure 3.8 |
Electron micrographs to show the arrangements of photoreceptor membranes in a vertebrate rod cell and an invertebrate rhabdom. 47 |
Figure 3.9 |
The structure of typical vertebrate photoreceptors illustrated diagrammatically, showing the inner and outer segments connected by a ciliary neck. 49 |
Figure 3.10 |
Structure of typical microvillar, or rhabdomeric, photoreceptors found in many invertebrates. 50 |
Figure 3.11 |
Optical modifications of vertebrate photoreceptors for specialized tasks. 51 |
Figure 3.12 |
Filters in vertebrate cones and the spectra of light they transmit. 52 |
Figure 3.13 |
Filters in invertebrate photoreceptors. 53 |
Figure 3.14 |
Photographs of invertebrate photoreceptor filter pigments and characteristic absorption spectra of the pigments. 54 |
Figure 3.15 |
Spectral sensitivity augmentation by sensitizing pigments. 55 |
Figure 3.16 |
Histograms to show values of λmax of visual pigments in photoreceptors of deep-sea animals. 57 |
Figure 3.17 |
Visual pigments in rod photoreceptors of marine mammals that reach different maximum depths when foraging, compared with a terrestrial mammal (the cow). 58 |
Figure 3.18 |
Irradiance spectra in typical habitats occupied by animals and an analysis of how photon capture in each irradiance condition changes with visual pigment λmax. 59 |
Figure 3.19 |
Color images of natural scenes in a temperate forest and a tropical coral reef along with dichromatic images of the same scenes using various wavelength pairs. 62 |
Figure 3.20 |
The cone array in the retina of the chinchilla (Chinchilla lanigera). 63 |
Figure 3.21 |
Developmental changes in visual pigments that occur during development of the hydrothermal vent crab Bythograea thermydron. 65 |
Figure 4.1 |
The pigment-pit eye of the arc clam Anadara notabilis. 67 |
Figure 4.2 |
A schematic camera eye viewing a visual scene (the trunk and bark of a eucalyptus tree). 69 |
Figure 4.3 |
Spatial resolution and light level. 70 |
Figure 4.4 |
Quantum events in photoreceptors. 72 |
Figure 4.5 |
Diffraction. 79 |
Figure 4.6 |
Optical aberrations. 81 |
Figure 4.7 |
Underfocused eyes. 83 |
Figure 4.8 |
Solutions for optical cross talk in the retina. 85 |
Figure 4.9 |
The modulation transfer function. 87 |
Figure 4.10 |
The modulation transfer function of an eye. 89 |
Figure 5.1 |
The pinhole eye of Nautilus. 93 |
Figure 5.2 |
The concave mirror eye of the scallop Pecten. 95 |
Figure 5.3 |
Camera eyes. 98 |
Figure 5.4 |
Amphibious vision in camera eyes. 101 |
Figure 5.5 |
The dark-adapted and light-adapted pupil of the nocturnal helmet gecko, Tarentola chazaliae. 103 |
Figure 5.6 |
The camera eyes of the octopus and the cod (Gadus morhua). 104 |
Figure 5.7 |
Ommatidia. 107 |
Figure 5.8 |
The two broad subtypes of compound eyes. 108 |
Figure 5.9 |
Focal and afocal optics in apposition compound eyes. 109 |
Figure 5.10 |
The three different types of superposition optics found in the Arthropoda, showing the paths of incident light rays focused by the cornea into the crystalline cones. 111 |
Figure 5.11 |
The refracting superposition eyes of the nocturnal dung beetle Onitis aygulus, which consist of a smaller dorsal eye and a larger ventral eye that are separated by a chitinous canthus on each side of the head. 112 |
Figure 5.12 |
Graded refractive index optics in the refracting superposition eyes of the nocturnal dung beetle Onitis aygulus. 113 |
Figure 5.13 |
Theoretical retinal image quality in the refracting superposition eyes of three species of dung beetles from the genus Onitis—the diurnal O. westermanni, the crepuscular O. alexis, and the nocturnal O. aygulus. The rhabdoms of dung beetles are flower shaped in cross section. 115 |
Figure 6.1 |
The definition of the interreceptor angle Δφ in camera eyes and the interommatidial angle Δφ in compound eyes. 118 |
Figure 6.2 |
The densities of rods and cones in the human retina as a function of distance (eccentricity) from the fovea. 120 |
Figure 6.3 |
The retinal ganglion cells, the sampling stations of the vertebrate retina. 122 |
Figure 6.4 |
The eyes of jumping spiders. 124 |
Figure 6.5 |
Negative lenses in camera eyes. 126 |
Figure 6.6 |
Acute zones and bright zones in apposition compound eyes. 128 |
Figure 6.7 |
An acute zone in the retina of a superposition compound eye. 130 |
Figure 6.8 |
Double eyes and sexual dimorphism in insects. 133 |
Figure 6.9 |
Optical sexual dimorphism in the apposition eyes of flies. 134 |
Figure 6.10 |
Neural sexual dimorphism in the apposition eyes of flies. 136 |
Figure 6.11 |
The dorsal acute zone of dragonflies. 138 |
Figure 6.12 |
The visual fields of predators and prey. 139 |
Figure 6.13 |
Horizontal acute zones in compound eyes. 140 |
Figure 6.14 |
Habitat-related ganglion cell topographies in terrestrial mammals. 141 |
Figure 6.15 |
Vision through Snel’s window, where the 180° view of the world above the water surface is compressed due to refraction into a 97.6°-wide cone below the water surface. 143 |
Figure 6.16 |
Deep-sea eye structure and the changing nature of visual scenes with depth. 144 |
Figure 7.1 |
An apple tree image optimized for human color vision using our three-color (RGB) printing process and with the color stripped from the image. 147 |
Figure 7.2 |
The varied spectral sensitivities of vertebrates and invertebrates. 148 |
Figure 7.3 |
The colors of natural objects and their perception by the color-vision systems of animals. 150 |
Figure 7.4 |
Histogram of the distribution of known spectral sensitivity peaks in 40 hymenopteran species. 153 |
Figure 7.5 |
The spectral sensitivities of tetrachromatic birds and fish. 154 |
Figure 7.6 |
The spectral sensitivities and resulting trichromatic color space of adult lungfish modeled without and with oil droplets. 155 |
Figure 7.7 |
Bee color vision and flower colors, reef fish color vision and fish colors. 158 |
Figure 7.8 |
UV patterns and contrast made visible to the human visual system by photography through UV filters. 160 |
Figure 7.9 |
λmax positions of cones and rods in marine fish arranged by habitat. 161 |
Figure 7.10 |
The spectral sensitivities of four species of reef fish from the same photic microhabitat. 162 |
Figure 7.11 |
Color vision in cardinal fish (Family Apogonidae). 163 |
Figure 7.12 |
African cichlid eyes collectively contain all seven cone opsins known in fish. 165 |
Figure 7.13 |
The offset hypothesis. 166 |
Figure 7.14 |
The barracuda Sphyraena helleri is associated with reefs and lives in a shallow photic habitat that could accommodate more than its known two spectral sensitivities. 167 |
Figure 7.15 |
Crustacean spectral sensitivities. 169 |
Figure 7.16 |
Spectral tuning with depth both between and within stomatopod species. 171 |
Figure 7.17 |
Behaviorally determined wavelength discrimination functions (Δλ) in stomatopods (Haptosquilla trispinosa) compared to various animals: human, goldfish, butterfly, honeybee. 172 |
Figure 7.18 |
Regionalization of spectral sensitivities in the eyes of vertebrates and invertebrates. 174 |
Figure 7.19 |
Vision in the archerfish (Toxoides chatareus), a fish that spits at prey located above the water surface. 175 |
Figure 7.20 |
The hypothetical origin of color vision as a way to remove caustic flicker produced by ripples in water in shallow Cambrian aquatic habitats. 176 |
Figure 8.1 |
Two representations of various states of polarized light starting as a nonpolarized light beam and its polarization through a linear polarizing filter and then conversion to circular polarization through a quarter-wave retarder. 179 |
Figure 8.2 |
Sources of linearly polarized light in nature. 181 |
Figure 8.3 |
Polarized light in air and water. 182 |
Figure 8.4 |
Sources of elliptically polarized light in nature. 184 |
Figure 8.5 |
The orientation of visual pigment chromophores and resulting dichroism within photoreceptor membrane. 185 |
Figure 8.6 |
Terrestrial and aquatic scenes showing differences in polarization information. 187 |
Figure 8.7 |
Drassodes cupreus and its polarization-sensitive posteromedial eyes. 188 |
Figure 8.8 |
Ommatidia in the dorsal rim area and main eye of various insects. 189 |
Figure 8.9 |
Rhabdom construction in crustaceans and cephalopods. 190 |
Figure 8.10 |
Circular polarization detection in stomatopods. 192 |
Figure 8.11 |
Responses of polarization receptors. 194 |
Figure 8.12 |
Polarization processing. 195 |
Figure 8.13 |
Polarization vision in the backswimmer Notonecta glauca. 196 |
Figure 8.14 |
Organization and function of dorsal rim area ommatidia. 197 |
Figure 8.15 |
Organization of polarization information coming from the compound eyes of the locust, Schistocerca gregaria. 198 |
Figure 8.16 |
Behavioral orientation and navigation under celestial e-vector patterns or artificial polarizers. 199 |
Figure 8.17 |
Responses of animals with polarization vision to virtual looming stimuli. 202 |
Figure 8.18 |
Choices of female Australian orchard swallowtail butterflies, Papilio aegeus, viewing color/polarization targets. 203 |
Figure 8.19 |
Linear polarization patterns likely to be involved in signaling. 204 |
Figure 9.1 |
Underwater photograph taken at the Great Barrier Reef off the coast of Australia and fog on a pond in Durham, NC. 207 |
Figure 9.2 |
A beam of light is both absorbed and scattered as it passes through an attenuating medium such as water or fog. 208 |
Figure 9.3 |
The scattering and beam attenuation coefficients at the surface and at 100 m depth in clear oceanic water. 210 |
Figure 9.4 |
A mackerel (Grammatorcynus bicarinatus) in water. The object, path, and total radiance for an object that is twice as bright as the background viewed horizontally in oceanic water at 480 nm. 212 |
Figure 9.5 |
The GretagMacbeth Colorchecker modeled to show how it would appear if viewed horizontally in clear oceanic water at a depth 5 m and at various viewing distances. 216 |
Figure 9.6 |
The minimum detectable radiance difference as a function of the adapting illumination in humans. 217 |
Figure 9.7 |
The contrast threshold as a function of the adapting illumination in humans. 218 |
Figure 9.8 |
The contrast threshold as a function of spatial frequency for a number of vertebrate species. 219 |
Figure 9.9 |
Historical room in Bäckaskog castle in Kristianstad, Sweden. 219 |
Figure 9.10 |
Sighting distances depend on the product of two factors. 221 |
Figure 9.11 |
The second factor affecting sighting distance (1/[c − KL cos θ]) is given in meters as a function of wavelength and viewing angle (θ) for oceanic water at 100-m depth. 222 |
Figure 9.12 |
The inherent contrast of an underwater object is greatly affected by wavelength. 223 |
Figure 9.13 |
Sighting distances for a large object that diffusely reflects 50% of the light that strikes it as a function of depth, wavelength, and viewing angle. 224 |
Figure 9.14 |
Underwater scene photographed through vertically oriented and horizontally oriented polarizers. Image also shown after polarization-based dehazing. 226 |
Figure 9.15 |
The scattering coefficients in a standard atmosphere as a function of altitude. 228 |
Figure 9.16 |
A case in which the only light that reaches the viewer’s eye is light that has never been scattered. Another case in which some of the light that reaches the viewer’s eye has been scattered more than once. An underwater scene where multiple scattering is just beginning to be significant. 230 |
Figure 10.1 |
Representations of flow fields projected onto a retina (these vectors also could represent the motions of objects in visual space). 234 |
Figure 10.2 |
A diagrammatic view of a Reichardt detector sensitive to motion in the direction of the large open arrow. An apparatus often used to study motion perception in flying insects. 236 |
Figure 10.3 |
Wide-field cells in the lobula plate of Drosophila. 238 |
Figure 10.4 |
Modeling wide-field sensing in flies. 238 |
Figure 10.5 |
Schematic showing typical responses of a motion-sensitive ganglion cell. 239 |
Figure 10.6 |
Motion sensing in vertebrate retinas. 239 |
Figure 10.7 |
Eye movements in mantis shrimp (photographs). 241 |
Figure 10.8 |
Eye movements in mantis shrimp (graphs). 242 |
Figure 10.9 |
Examples of optokinesis in aquatic animals. 245 |
Figure 10.10 |
Ocular fixation movements in action. 246 |
Figure 10.11 |
Tracking and chasing. 249 |
Figure 10.12 |
Tracking in praying mantis. 250 |
Figure 10.13 |
Flight room at the Janelia Farm research center in Virginia for study of the neurobiology underlying motion sensing and flight control in dragonflies. 252 |
Figure 10.14 |
Aerial tracking by a peregrine falcon at a cliff site in Colorado, showing spiral flight paths made to intercept prey over a lake. 253 |
Figure 10.15 |
Scanning behavior in larvae of the water beetle Thermonectes marmorata. 254 |
Figure 10.16 |
A flight tunnel used in experiments on motion vision of flying honeybees. 257 |
Figure 10.17 |
Schematic drawing of the apparatus used to test the abilities of honeybees to judge relative heights of “flowers” using motion vision. 258 |
Figure 10.18 |
Head movements of a foraging whooping crane searching for food on the ground. 259 |
Figure 10.19 |
Responses of a dragonfly interneuron that responds to looming stimuli, signaling estimated time to contact. 260 |
Figure 11.1 |
Nocturnal color vision in insects. 265 |
Figure 11.2 |
Nocturnal navigation using celestial cues. 267 |
Figure 11.3 |
Nocturnal homing in arthropods. 268 |
Figure 11.4 |
Visual acuity in nocturnal birds and mammals measured behaviorally as a function of luminance. 272 |
Figure 11.5 |
Optical adaptations for nocturnal vision in camera eyes. 274 |
Figure 11.6 |
Fresh head of a giant squid (likely from the genus Architeuthis). 275 |
Figure 11.7 |
Tubular eyes in deep-sea fish. 276 |
Figure 11.8 |
Rostral aphakic gaps in the eyes of deep-sea fish. 278 |
Figure 11.9 |
The tapetum lucidum and the eye glow of animal eyes. 279 |
Figure 11.10 |
Retinal specializations for dim-light vision in vertebrates. 280 |
Figure 11.11 |
The banked retina of the nocturnal oilbird and deep-sea fish. 281 |
Figure 11.12 |
Specializations for dim-light vision in compound eyes. 284 |
Figure 11.13 |
Spatial summation in nocturnal bees. 287 |
Figure 12.1 |
A sandhopper, Talitrus saltator, on a sandy beach in Italy. 290 |
Figure 12.2 |
Two photographs of deep-sea fish with bioluminescent lures and flashlight fish with lids open and closed. 292 |
Figure 12.3 |
The third zoeal stage of the larva of the crab Rhithropanopeus harrisi and graphs showing phototaxis to various wavelengths of light by early-stage zoea larvae and descent by these larvae on a sudden decrease in light intensity in clean water and in water that had contained ctenophores, a predator on larvae. 293 |
Figure 12.4 |
Genetic inheritance of orientation in different populations of Talitrus saltator on the west coast of Italy. 294 |
Figure 12.5 |
Orientations of ball-rolling dung beetles, Scarabaeus nigroaeneus, in the field, South Africa. 295 |
Figure 12.6 |
The orientations during flight outdoors near noon of intact and antennaless monarch butterflies, Danaus plexippus, before and after being subjected to a 6-hour phase delay in the light:dark cycle. 297 |
Figure 12.7 |
“Turn-back-and-look behavior” in foraging honeybees, Apis mellifera. 299 |
Figure 12.8 |
Nest orientation in the solitary wasp Cerceri rybyensis on leaving its nest hole. 300 |
Figure 12.9 |
Landmark learning in honeybees. 300 |
Figure 12.10 |
The “snapshot model” of landmark orientation in honeybees. 301 |
Figure 12.11 |
Nocturnal learning and recognition of landmarks by the Panamanian bee, Megalopta genalis. 302 |
Figure 12.12 |
Landmark orientation in the desert ant Cataglyphis bicolor, an individual of which is shown at the top left. 303 |
Figure 12.13 |
Place cells in the rat, Rattus norvegicus. 304 |
Figure 12.14 |
Landmark learning and use in the ant, Formica rufa. 306 |
Figure 12.15 |
Landmark use in the Australian desert ant, Melophorus bagoti. 307 |
Figure 12.16 |
Images of the Panamanian rainforest canopy taken at intervals along a trail through the forest. 308 |
Figure 12.17 |
Seaward orientation by hatchling loggerhead turtles, Caretta caretta. 309 |
Figure 12.18 |
Visual odometry in honeybees. 311 |
Figure 13.1 |
Silhouette of the cookie-cutter shark (Isistius brasiliensis) as viewed from below without counterillumination and the silhouette of the same animal with its many small ventral photophores turned on. 314 |
Figure 13.2 |
Animals in featureless environments, such as the snub-nosed dart (Trachinotus blochii), tend to employ strategies that reduce contrast and thus detectability. In contrast, animals in complex environments, such as the giant Australian cuttlefish (Sepia apama), tend to employ strategies that reduce recognition, usually via color patterns and in some cases texture. 315 |
Figure 13.3 |
The four primary mechanisms of camouflage and signaling are based on the four ways in which light can interact with matter. 316 |
Figure 13.4 |
The barreleye spookfish (Opisthoproctus soleatus), one of many deep-sea fish with eyes that look directly upward, presumably to catch the most light and thus improve vision. 317 |
Figure 13.5 |
The euphausiid shrimp, Meganyctiphanes norvegica, and the deep-sea hatchetfish, Argyropelecus aculeatus. 318 |
Figure 13.6 |
Various established and hypothesized functions of bioluminescence in marine species. 320 |
Figure 13.7 |
Fireflies at night. Note that the dark and simple background makes the signal easy to discern. 321 |
Figure 13.8 |
The bioluminescent courtship displays of various species of ostracod. 322 |
Figure 13.9 |
The coronate medusa Atolla wyvillei and one moment in the animal’s bioluminescent “pinwheel” display. 322 |
Figure 13.10 |
Examples of transparent animals. 324 |
Figure 13.11 |
Transparency and pelagic existence mapped onto a phylogeny of the major phyla in the Animalia. 325 |
Figure 13.12 |
The bolitaenid octopus, Japetella heathi in its transparent and pigmented forms. 327 |
Figure 13.13 |
The pelagic tunicate, Salpa cylindrical, and its transmittance of visible and ultraviolet radiation. 328 |
Figure 13.14 |
Selected images of transparent plankton viewed between parallel polarizing filters and crossed polarizers. 329 |
Figure 13.15 |
The anemone cleaner shrimp, Periclimenes holthuisi. 330 |
Figure 13.16 |
Strongly colored lures on the tips of the feeding tentacles of the siphonophore Resomia ornicephala, an example of aggressive mimicry. 330 |
Figure 13.17 |
Reflection from vertical mirrors underwater can look just like the view behind the animal because the light field is symmetric around the vertical axis. A vertical mirror photographed at 10-m depth shows that mirroring also works as crypsis in shallow waters if the sun is high in the sky. 331 |
Figure 13.18 |
The snub-nosed dart (Trachinotus blochii) well camouflaged by reflection from its silvery sides and a moment later, after it had tilted slightly, reflecting downwelling light to the viewer and becoming more visible. 332 |
Figure 13.19 |
The common bleak fish, Alburnus alburnus and a cross section of the fish showing the vertical orientation of most of the reflecting structures in the scales. 332 |
Figure 13.20 |
Intensity image of the bluefin trevally (Caranx melampygus), showing that it matches the background light well. Another image of the same fish shows the degree of polarization of the fish and the background. 334 |
Figure 13.21 |
Structural colors in the plumage of the male of the Indian peafowl (Pavo cristatus). 334 |
Figure 13.22 |
The coloration of oceanic species as a function of depth. 336 |
Figure 13.23 |
The red-eye gaper (Chaunax stigmaeus) photographed at depth under broad-spectrum lighting. 337 |
Figure 13.24 |
Mexican blind cavefish Astyanax mexicanus, one from a surface-dwelling population and one from a cave-dwelling population. 338 |
Figure 13.25 |
Twelve color morphs of the strawberry poison dart frog (Dendrobates pumilio), all from the Bocas del Toro region of Panama. 339 |
Figure 13.26 |
Maxwell triangle for a honeybee (Apis mellifera) viewing a violet flower and a blue flower through fog. 341 |
Figure 13.27 |
The dottyback (Pseudochromis paccagnellae) photographed at depth on a coral reef and a head of a male three-spined stickle-back (Gasterosteus aculeatus) photographed in the greenish water that it usually inhabits. 341 |
Figure 13.28 |
The GretagMacbeth ColorChecker™ as it appears in daylight and as it would appear if it were photographed at 40-m depth in clear ocean water or if it were photographed at 20-m depth in green coastal water. 342 |
Figure 13.29 |
Full-color image of two surf parrotfish (Scarus rivulatus), a lined butterflyfish (Chaetodon lienolatus), a minifin parrotfish (Scarus altipinnis), and the head of a hussar (Lutjanus adetii). The same image as viewed by an animal with only a medium-wavelength visual pigment, (the patterns on the three parrotfish are no longer visible) and as viewed by an animal with only a long-wavelength visual pigment, which increases the achromatic contrast of the details on the parrotfish. A dichromatic viewer can distinguish more than a monochromat. 343 |