List of Figures

Figure 1.1 Descartes' model of how an external event might
cause bodily movement.
Figure 1.2 A simple reflex and its connections to the spinal cord.
Figure 1.3 A typical arrangement for studying salivation in a
conditioning experiment in Pavlov's 19th century
laboratory.
Figure 1.4 Thorndike's puzzle box.
Figure 1.5 A Skinner box.
Figure 2.1 Essential features of a Skinner box for a rat, or other
small rodent.
Figure 2.2 Event records, or "time lines", illustrating a
contingency between, or independence of, Event A
and Event B.
Figure 2.3 A cumulative recorder of a type that was used
extensively until replaced by computer software
systems.
Figure 2.4 Cumulative records obtained frofn four rats on their first
session of operant conditioning.
Figure 2.5 Relative frequencies of several behaviors occurring
in a Skinner box before and after operant conditioning
of lever pressing.
Figure 2.6 Distribution of response forces when (A, upper graph)
all responses with a force of more than 21 g were
reinforced, and (B, lower graph) when all responses
with a force of more than 36 g were reinforced.
Figure 3.1 Cumulative record of responding in extinction of lever
press response previously reinforced with food.
Figure 3.2 Cumulative records of lever pressing for two rats
reinforced with food 100 times and then transferred
to a response independent food presentation schedule.
Figure 3.3 Apparatus used by Antonitis (1951) to reinforce nose
poking.
Figure 3.4 Apparatus used for measuring aggression induced by
extinction in a Skinner box.
Figure 3.5 Resistance to extinction of lever pressing as a
function of the weight of the lever (or bar).
Figure 3.6 Spontaneous recovery from extinction of a rat's
lever-press response.
Figure 3.7 Averaged cumulative response curves for the first (1),
fifth (5), and tenth (10) sessions of extinction.
Figure 3.8 Average data for classical conditioning followed by
extinction of rabbits' nictitating membrane response.
Figure 3.9 Typical cumulative records of performances
maintained by four schedules of intermittent
reinforcement.
Figure 4.1 Data of Pavlov (1927) on the development of
differentiation, or discrimination, by two individual
dogs in salivary conditioning experiments.
Figure 4.2 A pigeon Skinner box fitted with an optical system to
project pure light on to the pecking key.
Figure 4.3 Numbers of key pecking responses emitted by pigeons
in the presence of 11 different wavelengths of light,
projected one at a time on to the pecking key.
Figure 4.4 Van Houten and Rudolph's data showing generaliza-
tion gradients for pigeons that had been trained in the
presence of 1000 Hz auditory frequency, either in
the dark (no key light) or with an illuminated key.
Figure 4.5 Cumulative records of lever pressing by a rat on a
multiple FI 5-minute FR20 schedule (upper panel),
and on a multiple FI 5-minute FR40 schedule (lower
panel).
Figure 4.6 A two-key pigeon Skinner box.
Figure 4.7 Cumulative records of key pecking by a pigeon a
concurrent FR100 FI 5-minute schedule of
reinforcement.
Figure 4.8 Inhibitory generalization gradients for groups of
pigeons on successive test days with VI reinforcement
at all line-tilt stimulus values.
Figure 4.9 Schematic diagram of a typical layout of stimuli
presented simultaneously (or with onset of A1 slightly
before the other stimuli) on a computer screen for
matching-to sample training of the A-B relationship.
Figure 4.10 Outcome of stimulus equivalence class training.
Figure 5.1 A shuttle box designed to study aversive contingencies
with rats.
Figure 5.2 Escape response rates as a function of the intensity
of three different aversive stimuli.
Figure 5.3 Event records or timelines illustrating the procedures
of (A) free operant avoidance; (b) discriminated
avoidance; and (c) escape.
Figure 5.4 Three measures of behavior during acquisition of a
discriminated lever-press avoidance response.
Figure 5.5 Cumulative records of lever press avoidance during
training of a rat on free operant avoidance.
Figure 5.6 Cumulative records of the punished responding
of human participants on a VI schedule of reinforce-
ment under three conditions.
Figure 5.7 Response rate, with and without a punishment
contingency for three human experimental participants
as a function of reinforcement rate in the components
of a multiple VI schedule.
Figure 5.8 Cumulative records showing developments of
conditional suppression in a rat lever pressing for water
reinforcement on a variable-interval schedule.
Figure 6.1 The Wisconsin general test apparatus.
Figure 6.2 Changes in rate of acquisition of discrimination
processes.
Figure 6.3 Development of a learning set.
Figure 6.4 Performance of five species on a series of visual
discrimination problems.
Figure 6.5 Patterns of geometric symbols grouped according to a
two-out-of-three polymorphous rule.
Figure 6.6 A set of cards used to study concept identification.
Figure 6.7 Average number of aggressive acts modeled by
children as a function of consequences for the model,
sex of child, and whether the child was reinforced for
modeling.
Figure 6.8 Reinforced imitative and nonreinforced imitative
responding by a single child during a sequence of
different reinforcement conditions.
Figure 6.9 The behavior analytic account of dialog is like a tennis
match with each person taking turns as speaker
and listener.
Figure 6.10 An example of rule following.
Figure 7.1 A model of the behavior and environmental conditions
to be assessed.
Figure 7.2 Example of interval recording.
Figure 7.3 Example of interval recording using multiple
behaviors.
Figure 7.4 Example of interval recording with multiple persons.
Figure 7.5 Example of a scatterplot grid over a five-day period.
Figure 7.6 Functional analysis results of self-injury for two
individuals.
Figure 8.1 hypothetical example of a single-case design graph
depicting the major features of graphic display.
Figure 8.2 Hypothetical example of a stable data path (Graph A)
and variable data points (Graph B).
Figure 8.3 Stable baseline data (Graph A) allow for a clear
interpretation of the effectiveness of treatment in the
intervention phase.
Figure 8.4 There is a clear change in level between the last data
point of the baseline phase and the first data point of
the intervention phase in Graph A.
Figure 8.5 An increasing or decreasing trend in baseline data
paths can be problematic if the subsequent interven-
tion is designed to increase (Graph A) or decrease
(Graph B) the target behavior respectively.
Figure 8.6 Percentage of intervals of stereotypy during transition
between work activities for a man with autism.
Figure 8.7 Number of accident days per week for an adolescent
girl with diurnal enuresis.
Figure 8.8 The number of self-identified designated drivers per
evening during weekend evenings in a college campus
bar.
Figure 8.9 An example of an ABABCBC design.
Figure 8.10 Percentage of correctly asking the question "What's
that?" when pointing to a novel stimulus across
baseline and instruction phases of a multiple baseline
design for three students with autism.
Figure 8.11 The effects of a public posting intervention.
Figure 8.12 Percentage of intervals in which appropriate and
inappropriate turn waiting, initiating interactions, and
interacting with others was observed for a class of
students with severe/profound hearing loss.
Figure 8.13 Use of a changing criterion design to systematically
increase work units per minute for workers with
developmental disabilities.
Figure 8.14 Total seconds of eye poking during four assessment
conditions presented in an alternating treatments
design.
Figure 8.15 The effects of experimenter-selected versus subject-
selected reinforcers on responses per minute for four
individuals with developmental disabilities.
Figure 9.1 Percentage of time that four participants engaged or
manipulated items that were identified as preferred in
the forced-choice assessment and preference
assessment versus stimuli that were identified as highly
preferred in the preference assessment only.
Figure 9.2 Percentage of intervals of problem and desirable
classroom behavior for three students with emotional
and behavioral problems under preferred and non-
preferred curricular activities.
Figure 9.3 Daily mean percentage (N=10) of participation in
exercises and games and the exercise session length
during baseline and group contingency phases.
Figure 9.4 Responses per minute for three participants when no
food (reinforcement) was presented (baseline
conditions in the graph) and when food was presented
contingent on responding under satiation and
deprivation conditions.
Figure 9.5 Total number of hours each day a group of 44 clients
participated in rehabilitative activities under
conditions of reinforcement with tokens (first phase),
independent or non-contingent presentations of tokens
(second phase), and reinstatement of reinforcement
with tokens (third phase).
Figure 9.6 Episodes of sleeping in class under baseline and DRI
(differential reinforcement of academic performance)
contingencies.
Figure 9.7 Hypothetical example of a brief functional analysis.
Figure 9.8 The rate of self-injury for Brenda.
Figure 9.9 Rate of eating across four participants under baseline
and treatment conditions. Open data points represent
data for one meal.
Figure 9.10 A comparison of a lean (FT 5 min) versus dense (FT 10
s) schedule of noncontingent reinforcement on
destructive responses per minute with quadruplets.
Figure 9.11 Delayed prompting.
Figure 9. 12 Daily percentages of opportunities used by teachers to
delay and by children to initiate, before (baseline) and
after (intervention) teachers programmed delays with
each of the six children.
Figure 9.13 An example of stimulus fading in which the stimulus
prompt is gradually faded out.
Figure 9.14 An example of stimulus fading in which additional
stimulus prompts are superimposed on the natural
discriminative stimuli and gradually faded during
instruction.
Figure 9.15 An example of stimulus shaping to teach word
identification.
Figure 9.16 Percentage of independent mealtime skills for four
individuals with profound developmental disabilities.
Figure 9.17 Cumulative number of untrained play topographies
across toys for two children.
Figure 9.18 Conversational initiations per minute by participants to
partners with and without disabilities across school
settings.
Figure 10. 1 Duration of asthmatic responding at bedtime during
baseline and extinction phases.
Figure 10. 2 Responses per minute of self-injurious behavior during
baseline when attention is delivered contingent on SIB
and during NCR intervention (phase 2 in the graph)
when attention is delivered noncontingently.
Figure 10.3 Frequency of night wakings for seven children under an
extinction intervention. The large solid dots
represent nights in which the infant was ill.
Figure 10.4 Rate of self-injurious behavior (upper panel) and aggressive
responses (lower panel) for an individual
under baseline and extinction intervention.
Figure 10.5 The number of face slaps per minute when wrist
weights were on and when they were off.
Figure 10.6 Percentage of intervals containing SIB and object
manipulation during baseline (BL) and across effort
(string-length) conditions for three participants.
Figure 10.7 Percentage of intervals of SIB maintained by sensory
consequences.
Figure 10.8 Rates of self-choking by a deaf-blind man with mental
retardation before and during water mist treatment and
its generalization and follow-up.
Figure 10.9 Frequency of aggressive behavior of a young girl
towards her younger sibling under baseline, DRO, and
DRO plus verbal reprimand conditions.
Figure 10.10 Number of disruptions and aggressive behaviors per
child per hour for 50 days in a day care center with
follow-up at 1 and 2 months.
Figure 10.11 Number of arm bites (top panel) and mouthing
incidents (bottom panel) under baseline, DRO alone,
and movement suppression time-out.
Figure 10.12 Percentage of intervals with stereotypy across baseline,
DRO, and time-out conditions in task (top panel) and
leisure (bottom panel) contexts.
Figure 10.13 Number of stealing episodes each day for a group of 34
persons with developmental disabilities in an
institutional setting.
Figure 10.14 Number of hits per day during school period.
Figure 11.1 Daily corrected percentage of sales for all salads during
baseline and intervention (posters etc.) in a restaurant.
Figure 11.2 Mean revolutions pedaled per minute during baseline,
VR 1 (VR range, 70 to 85), VR 2 (VR range, 90 to 115),
VR 3 (VR range, 100 to 130), return to baseline,
and return to VR 3 phases for obese and nonobese
boys.
Figure 11.3 Percentage of contact situations in which gloves were
worn by 4 nurses for consecutive 10-hr shifts.
Figure 11.4 Percentage of correct responses on simulation and self-
administration probes for four children who were
taught self-care of tracheotomies.
Figure 11.5 The yearly number of days lost from work (top graph)
and work-related injuries (bottom graph), per million
person hours worked under baseline and token
economy conditions in both mines.
Figure 11.6 The percentage of R1 responses as a function of the
percentage of reinforcements for that response
alternative.
Figure 11.7 Amount of eye contact as a function of rate of social
reinforcement.
Figure 11.8 Percentage of time that one experimental participant,
Matt, allocated to performing arithmetic problems on
the richer reinforcement schedule.
Figure 11.9 Pigeons trained to match A1 to B1 and A2 to B2, and
to match B1 to C1 and B2 to C2, were tested for
transitivity by presenting A1 or A2 as sample stimuli
and C1 and C2 as comparison stimuli.